Critique: Report of the APA Task Force
on the Sexualization of Girls (2007)
Arnold Veraa, PhD*
ABSTRACT: This review assesses the evidence this report quotes in
support of its conclusion that the sexualization of “girls” is
widespread and that it has damaging consequences. It was found
that the report relies heavily and inappropriately on adolescent and
adult data to support this contention. No direct evidence was
detected in this report that supports the claims that “girls” may suffer
from self-image problems, eating disorders, depression or an inability
to develop a healthy sexuality as a result of “sexualization”.
The Report and its Summary Conclusions:
Following a recommendation by the American
Psychological Association’s “Committee on Women in Psychology” a Task
Force was established in February 2005 to report on the sexualization of
girls.
This Task Force reported in 2007 and reached the
conclusions, in brief, that the sexualization of girls leads to
undermining persons’ emotional and self-image, eating disorders, low
self-esteem, depression in girls and women and has negative consequences
on girls’ ability to develop a healthy sexual self-image.
The Report’s Definition of
“Sexualization” (page 2):
The report identifies sexualization as occurring
when a person’s value is estimated on the basis of their sexual appeal
or attractiveness, or when persons are “sexually objectified” (made into
a “thing” for others’ sexual gratification).
The report advises inappropriately imposed
sexuality is particularly relevant to children's sexualization, as they
are then imbued with adult sexuality which is forced, rather than chosen
by them.
Particular examples mentioned in the Introduction
(page 2) are infants or elementary school age girls being subjected by
toy manufacturers to dolls featuring miniskirts and thigh-high boots,
clothing stores selling thongs marked “eye candy” or “wink wink,” and
beauty pageants in which they wear makeup, false eyelashes, and hair
extensions and are encouraged to act flirtatiously onstage.
Other examples mentioned concern girls wearing
T-shirts that say “flirt,” or instructions given by magazines for
preadolescent girls on ways to look sexy. As well, we are asked to
think of ads that portray women as little girls.
The report thus encourages the reader to think the
content is indeed going to be about “girls” (as the authors use the term
in the title and in the Introduction) in this age range rather than
about adolescents or adults. It is important to keep this in mind.
A Troublesome Definition:
The authors’ use of the term is vexatious in that
a purported community problem – sexualization of girls – is only
identified in relation to the moral attitudes and perceptions of the
principal feminist psychologist authors and reviewers (33 in all, of
whom one is apparently a male) and some of the references they chose to
quote.
The authors make no attempt to put child
sexualization into a societal perspective. Until results of
comprehensive community prevalence research about attitudes, prevalence
and possible effects about child sexualization are available, opinions
and attitudes expressed about the topic can only be those of the
proponents and cannot be generalized from.
Given the nature of the topic, which is
essentially one of moral or attitudinal preferences towards the
expression of childhood sexuality, such an open-ended definition invites
discord and is likely to be divisive in relation to personal beliefs or
ideological opinions held.
It seems contentious to claim, on behalf of a
professional association, that a “girl sexualization problem” exists
without demonstrating a community consensus about the issue, and some
agreement among psychologists, and allied professions, that we indeed
have such a concern. The report does not supply such assurance.
The Report’s use of “Girls”:
We must acknowledge the term “girl” can be
equivocal. Colloquially, it might refer to children, adolescents,
or adult women. Even in professional literature, adolescents are
often referred to as “girls” and both children and adolescents might be
ambiguously lumped together as “girls”.
However, in the context of this report it is
evident what it means by “girls” and that is that they are of the infant
and childhood variety as outlined in the Introduction (page 2).
(The perception that the report is about children, rather than about
adolescents or adults, appears to have contributed to its appeal).
The report thus confirms accepted psychological
nomenclature: Infants are those of about two to five and children are
those of about six to twelve years; the appropriate age range for girls.
It is generally accepted in literature that young persons above this age
are referred to as adolescents, and, after seventeen or eighteen, are
considered as adults.
One of the most frustrating aspects of this report
is its tendency to confound age ranges in an effort to find support for
the notion that girls are adversely affected by being sexualised.
(This does not appear to be the effect of possible confusion about age
ranges in the literature quoted in the report).
Frequently, the authors obliquely quote evidence
as relevant to girls when it is only derived from data pertinent to
adolescents or adults. Authors repeatedly attempt to associate
girls with the experiences of adolescents and women by using such
phrases as “girls and teens,” and “girls and young women.”
Three quotes from the report illustrate this
disturbing trend to link girls or children with adolescents or adults
which confound any meaningful information about girls, the purported
subject of the report:
“… The recent and global proliferation of Web sites of all kinds
that may be of interest to girls makes it difficult to assess the
sexualization of girls and women. Lambiase (2003) examined the
sexualization of girls and women on one specific type of Web site
that targets and attracts girls and teens …” (page 11).
“… Research links sexualization with three of the most common
mental health problems of girls and women: eating disorders; low
self-esteem; depression or depressed mood …” (page 24).
“… If the idealized female sexual partner is a 15- or 16-year–old
girl, male consumers may demand pornography featuring such girls and
the opportunity to pay for sex with them. A 2005 report noted
a disturbing new trend in the recruitment of children into
pornography: Pedophiles and “johns” look in online chat rooms for
teens …” (page 35)
This perplexing manner of writing, turning
children into teens or adults to the report’s advantage, appears a way
of avoiding the embarrassing conclusion the authors refuse to face: that
they have been unable to provide valid evidence for the notion that
“girls” are being sexualized en mass, and that sexualization, as they
define it, produces negative effects.
But let us review the evidence.
“Evidence for the Sexualization of Girls”:
(page 5)
Under this main heading, the report discusses
evidence for the existence of the phenomenon of “girl sexualization.”
It does this under a number of topics ranging from cultural
contributions (such as the media or music), advertising and products, to
interpersonal and intrapsychic contributions.
Under this heading, and in the Introduction, the
authors mention some 239 references. Of these, roughly 56% (133)
refer to adults, approximately 27% (66) refer to adolescents or partly
so, and 17% (40) may be said to relate to infants and children.
The authors’ admission in the report, that much of
the research reviewed concerns the sexualization of women seems more
than confirmed. The realization that only 17% of these references
actually concern infants and children – the “girls” the report refers to
in its introduction – is disappointing.
More than a quarter of these 17% of references
relate to newspaper articles or other popular media. While such
reports may appear insightful and informative to the populace, they do
tend to rely on opinion and selected information. Genuine research
eschews such information. However, this report accepts it and
quotes such material as evidence for child sexualization.
The remaining three quarters of these references
quoted in favour of the existence of the sexualization of infants and
children consists of sixteen articles in professional journals, nine
books, and four articles in books. While many of these references
employ research (using a generous definition) none inform us as to what
the American community actually conceives child sexualization to be, or
what the extent of it may be.
The authors themselves, however, make it quite
clear what they think child sexualization entails. They see it as
the eroticization of little girls, their fetishization, and the
exploitation of their innocence and vulnerability. Portrayals of
under-age nymphets and their non-threatening nature are seen to
illustrate accessibilities and possibilities of seduction designed to
fuel male desires, and pedophile tendencies in particular. Authors
advance the notion that such sexualization attempts are then deceivingly
presented as natural, or as unremarkable in the media.
Particular examples of premature sexualization of
girls in these publications may refer to how dolls are dressed in
sexually alluring clothing, how sexually provocative clothing for
children is advertised (padded bras, thong panties, or revealing T
shirts for example) and how children themselves are encouraged to pose
in seductive and erotic ways in advertising, films and on the internet.
Other examples of sexualization quoted may refer
to how young girls are encouraged to wear make-up, perfumes or other
beauty enhancements usually considered more appropriate for adolescents
or women. (See Appendix A for authors quoted by the report in
these 17% of references).
Such behaviors and practices however, can only be
seen as supporting the authors theory of sexualization of girls if one
happens to agree they actually constitute sexualized behaviours.
By the report’s own estimation, such practices seem reasonably
widespread, and it may therefore be assumed that a sizeable portion of
American children and adults do not share these authors’ apparent belief
that such displays are immodest, indecent, or generally deviant and
socially unacceptable.
As the report presents no evidence drawn from
community prevalence attitudinal research, no conclusion can be reached
about girl sexualization being a significant problem or concern in
society.
The research quoted in these 17% of references
also advises about ways gender differences are constructed in children,
and how this may be reinforced and maintained. We hear about sex
typing in leisure activities, how friendship or popularity may play a
role in the construction of girlhood, how girls may compete with each
other, and how socialization experiences can heighten social power for
boys.
At best, such research is only remotely relevant
to the topic at hand, which is to establish evidence for the existence
of the sexualization of girls.
In all, the report presents only quite limited
evidence about the contention that certain behaviours, which the authors
believe constitute ‘sexualization’, actually present a problem for a
significant number of girls in American society.
The views of those who might interpret such
behaviors differently are not considered. The opinions of the
authors, and those of the selected references quoted, can thus not be
generalised from, and the proposition that ‘girl sexualization’ is a
significant social problem has therefore not been established.
“Consequences of the Sexualization of Girls”: (page 19)
Under this main heading, the
report discusses evidence for the negative effects of “girl
sexualization”. Some theories, and the impacts on girls’ health
and well-being, sexuality, attitudes and beliefs, effects of sexual
exploitation on girls, and the impact on others appear under subsequent
subheadings.
The authors state there are
many “negative consequences” to such sexualization, and cite some 262
references in this section. Of these, roughly 59% (155) refer to
adults, approximately 26% (69) refer to adolescents or partly so, and
15% (38) may be said to relate to infants and children.
It is, again, disconcerting to
find in this report, which claims to be about “girls”, only 15% of the
references actually refer to infants and children.
Of these 15% quoted in favour
of negative consequences of the sexualization of girls, 20 refer to
articles in professional journals, six refer to books, five to articles
in books, and two refer to reports. (The authors of these publications
are presented in Appendix B).
In this section of the report,
where one expects to find evidence for the ill effects of the
sexualization of girls, “theories” are first referred to (page 20).
Surprisingly few actually refer to infants and children, and those that
do appear not to be particularly relevant to the topic under discussion.
Some articles referred to
discuss psychoanalytic theory in depth and others are philosophical in
nature, or discuss therapeutic issues. Others relevant to children
refer to gender-stereotypical preferences, the origin and development of
implicit attitudes, and how cognitive developmental factors may
influence self-presentations, self-conscious emotions, self-recognition
and self-awareness.
Further articles about children
discuss how girls and boys may develop differently, how body image and
self-esteem may grow and evolve, how math and science participation may
predict social expectancies and values, and how girls who focus less on
appearance may perform better academically.
While such discourse provides
interesting theoretical perspectives, some of which is backed by
research, it does not provide evidence for the “many negative
consequences” this report claims exists about the sexualization of
girls.
The authors’ tendency to
interpret theories as evidence (“objectification theory” being an
obvious example), are inappropriate. Theories may helpfully
explain and interpret events and behaviours. But, typically, they
do not provide information about the prevalence or measured effects of a
given phenomenon; in this case the possible existence and effects of the
sexualization of girls (one research paper attempts to support the
objectification theory in relation to adolescents).
Under the heading “Cognitive
and Physical Functioning” (page 22) five references relate to children.
They refer to analyses of physical movements, to lateral movements and
how girls “throw” differently from boys, how physical activity is
beneficial for girls, how math and science participation may predict
social expectancies and values, and how girls who focus less on
appearance may perform better academically.
The discussion under the
heading “Body Dissatisfaction and Appearance Anxiety” (page 23) mentions
just one reference relevant to infants. It is a theoretical thesis
on self-conscious emotions, self-awareness and how embarrassment may
arise after self-recognition develops. This reference contains no
evidence concerning the effects of sexualization.
Under the heading “Mental
Health” (page 24) the authors discuss eating disorders, low self-esteem
and depression as undesirable effects of sexualization. There are
no references here that relate to infants or children, but the authors
clearly infer that the findings are also applicable to “girls.”
The same applies to the heading “Physical Health” (page 25) which it is
claimed may also suffer from sexualization.
The report even manages not to
mention any references about infants and children when it claims, under
the heading “Sexuality”, that “ … the sexualization of girls has
negative consequences on girls’ ability to develop healthy sexuality …”
(page 26). For those who believe child sexualization is a problem
this is an issue that would concern them most. Yet, the authors
quote no more than six references concerning adolescents, and 23
references concerning adults to support their view that “girls” are at
risk in this way.
Under the heading “Attitudes
and Beliefs” (page 27) the authors seek to provide evidence for the
notion that the media sexualizes girls, and that they are adversely
affected by viewing such exposures. Again, they provide no
statistical support for their belief such practices affect infants and
children and no references concerning this age range are provided.
The discussion about the impact
of girls’ sexualization continues with the heading “Effects of Sexual
Exploitation on Girls” (p 28). The authors state that childhood
sexual abuse victimization is an extreme form of sexualization and that
a review of its sequelae may be relevant to understanding the effects of
child sexualization.
This attempt to link child
sexual abuse and child exploitation with sexualization is unfortunate.
The report quotes eight references in this section that refer to
children and the adverse effects they may suffer as a result of child
sexual abuse, but none of those suggest these are due to sexualization
as this report defines it.
It is quite inappropriate to
suggest that the possible negative effects of sexualization concerning
children, of which this report has not provided valid or reliable
evidence, are in any way comparable to the effects children experience
after sexual abuse or exploitation.
This prompts the observation
that this kind of rationalization is a feminist tactic of old.
Feminists in earlier decades employed similar means to increase
awareness and seriousness of child sexual abuse by widening its
definition. “Father-daughter rape”, for instance, became common
feminist folklore when “sexual abuse,” “sexual molestation” or “sexual
assault” explained the adult-child sexual activity more accurately.
Particularly, since rape was found to be a rare occurrence in these
circumstances.
It appears current advocates of
child sexualization are engaging in similar exaggerations and
misjudgements.
Continuing with the headings in
this section that supposedly provide evidence for the ill effects of
sexualization concerning “girls”, the heading “Boys and Men” (page 29)
has only two references concerning children, and these are about boys,
not girls.
The heading “Women” (page 29)
contains two references that concern children, but the first is about
how marketing and merchandising may create female consuming subjects,
and the second relates to an unreferenced author (Moore, 2003) advising
that children of 11 or 12 years have achieved modelling success.
While these references could possibly provide limited support for the
authors’ perception of sexualization, they do not provide evidence for
its negative effects.
Under “Impact on Society” (page
31) the report states “ … In addition to the serious consequences for
girls that have been outlined previously …” (note the word “girls”) yet,
we have just shown that the report has not presented any conclusive
evidence for adverse effects concerning girl infants and children so
far. Such misleading comments appear with regularity in this
report and are likely to lead the unsuspecting reader to believe
reliable evidence has been presented when it has not.
The report claims sexualization
of girls is likely to have “numerous negative consequences for society”,
and under the heading “Sexism” (page 31) we find the usual feminist
concerns expressed about sex bias, sexist attitudes, sexual harassment,
sex role stereotypes, and how the media may reinforce such attitudes by
featuring women as sexual objects.
While such sentiments may have
widespread feminist acceptance, this claim seems unnecessarily ambitious
when used as evidence for the long term adverse effects of child
sexualization (as defined in the Introduction). Confirmation would
require substantial retrospective research which this report does not
present.
The report quotes one reference
in this section relevant to children (about sexual portrayals of girls
in advertising using four fashion ads). While this reference
argues firmly that sexualization may have implications for children’s
psychological and physical well-being, it supplies no reliable evidence
for this, or for the notion that child sexualization may have long term
effects. (The relevant reference is Merskin, 2004).
Discussion under the heading
“Girls’ Educational Success and Achievement” (page 33) suggests that, as
girls’ preoccupation with appearance ties up cognitive resources, this
limits educational and occupational opportunities. Possibly, it
may for some, but the report does not provide evidence this is related
to the sexualization of children; no references in this section relate
to them.
The same applies to “Sexual
Harassment in School” (page 33). There are no references about the
possible ill effects of sexualization of infants and children or their
harassment here, either. Yet, the report claims it has evidence
that “ … the sexualization of girls contributes to sexual harassment and
coercion …” (page 33) with subsequent references only referring to
adolescents and adults and no retrospective research being quoted.
The last two headings in this
report that attempt to provide evidence for the negative effects of girl
sexualization are “Violence Against Girls” and “Sexual Exploitation”.
(Interestingly, the report already had an earlier heading in this
section called “Effects of Sexual Exploitation on Girls” (page 28).
A reader may query this emphasis).
Under the heading “Violence
Against Girls” (page 34) the report discusses the possible relationship
of sexist beliefs and media exposure with violence against women (as it
did under “Sexism”, page 31). It quotes one study as having used
“children” to support this proposition, but the findings of this
particular research can more appropriately be considered as relevant to
adolescents (boys and girls 11-16 ).
Even then, the research can not
be interpreted as proposing that girl sexualization actually promotes
violence against them. The suggestion that it might is quite
inappropriate and seems yet another example of the authors’ emotive
over-generalisations.
The last heading, “Sexual
Exploitation”, (page 34) also provides no evidence for any possible
adverse effects of girl sexualization. There are two references
that relate to children; the first was already referred to above, and
concerns sexual portrayal of girls in advertising. This reference
may provide a modicum of support for the existence of child
sexualization (as the authors view it) in fashion advertisements but
cannot be quoted as providing evidence for any measurable ill effects.
The second reference is also
about how children may be viewed as sexual objects in magazines.
However, since this paper found that only 1.5% of ads in five magazines
depicted children sexually, this article may have been better left
unquoted as this finding does not appear to provide support for the
report’s contention that girl sexualised advertising is rife. (The
authors of this reference are O’Donohue et al, 1997).
Thus, in all, under the major
heading, “Consequences of the Sexualization of Girls”, which led us to
believe it would provide evidence for the adverse effects of
sexualization upon “girls”, the authors have presented none.
The references that actually
were about infants and children contained no evidence concerning the
report’s main claims that “girls” may suffer from eating disorders, low
self-esteem or depression, or that sexualization may have negative
consequences on girls’ ability to develop a healthy sexual self-image.
As the report presents no
findings concerning large scale retrospective research, it also can not
claim that possible effects adolescents and adults may experience today
have their foundation in infancy or childhood.
It is clear, by the suggestive
discussion in this report, that the authors firmly believe that there
ought to be adverse consequences of “sexualization” for girls.
However, they have comprehensively failed to provide evidence for this
belief.
Report’s Rationalization for Using Adult
Studies:
While the report offers no
apologies for its frequent misuse of the word “girls”, and the
misleading connotations and misinterpretations this leads to, it does
refer to the paucity of research on the sexualization of girls.
The report acknowledges that “ … much of the research reviewed in this
report concerns the sexualization of women …”, (page 4) but rationalizes
this as being appropriate by saying that this “research” is highly
relevant to the sexualization of girls because of developmental and
methodological “arguments”.
The developmental “argument”
revolves around the notion that girls develop their identities by
modelling themselves on sexualized women who, themselves, have been
influenced by sexualized images found in the media. The report
concludes that the sexualization of women is, therefore, highly relevant
to the sexualization of girls.
The methodological “argument”
suggests that people develop particular perspectives after having been
exposed to consistent themes over time. Hence, a lifetime of
exposure to sexualized images, the authors argue, is bound to affect
young women. How women feel about sexualization is therefore
“entirely relevant” (page 4) to understanding how “girls” feel about,
and respond to, sexualization today.
These two over-generalisations
are offered as a rationale for the extensive use of adolescent and adult
material to explain the possible existence and effects of sexualization
on “girls.” Such a manner of analysis would be troublesome at the
best of times, but this report does not even establish, as we have seen,
that the American community as a whole perceives its girls to be
sexualised, or that it may produce negative effects in the first place.
The references concerning
adolescents and adults were not reviewed in the same detail, but it
appears that the findings quoted seem also unlikely to fairly reflect
community attitudes, experiences and effects. If that assumption
has any validity, it is another reason to resist the temptation of
assuming that adolescents and adults routinely influence girls adversely
in matters concerning their sexual expression.
Concerning the Report’s “Recommendations”:
(page 42)
Below this heading, the report
discusses recommendations under “Research”, “Practice”, “Education and
Training”, “Public Policy” and “Public Awareness”.
The authors give the impression
that these recommendations are based on solid research and sound review
of the literature about the sexualization of “girls”. We have
demonstrated, however, that this report has not presented valid and
reliable evidence concerning the wholesale sexualization of “girls” and
the alleged negative effects.
The recommendations, therefore,
present more as an ideological wish list, not necessarily inappropriate
per se (particularly when issues of sex equality are considered) but
based on values and attitudes concerning child sexuality others may not
share. The recommendations may be comforting for those who already
“believe in the cause”, but as suggestions for further action based on
reliable research go, they seem based on untested assumptions.
Policy makers, practitioners,
teachers, and also parents, might exercise caution in seeking to
implement these recommendations, keeping in mind no valid evidence has
been presented that “girls” (children) in general are adversely affected
by “sexualization.”
Differing opinions and
attitudes about behaviors and practices these authors consider to be
“sexualised” should be respected unless it can be conclusively
demonstrated that they are harmful to girls (children). The
authors have not presented evidence they are.
Conclusion:
This report’s attempt to present “girl sexualization” as a
significant social problem has not succeeded.
First, because the feminist psychologist authors
have not demonstrated that their views about the expression of child
sexuality are representative of American society, of psychologists, or
those of other professions. Relevant community prevalence research
may not yet be available, but the writers do not even consider the
possibility that others may not share their opinions, values and
attitudes about “girl sexualization.”
Second, the authors fail to provide valid and
reliable evidence for the supposed problem. Only fragmented
research support is provided for the notion that girl sexualization is
relevant or exists to a significant degree in the community (and that is
dependent on what one believes “sexualization” to be), and no direct
evidence is supplied that this may have negative effects on “girls”
(children). The report’s attempts to relate adolescent and adult
findings to the reactions of children is untenable.
This, together with the deceptive style of
writing, makes this report unworthy to have been published on behalf of
the American Psychological Association.
APPENDIX A
References judged to be broadly relevant to infants and children as
mentioned under the headings “Introduction” and “Evidence for the
Sexualization of Girls”:
Adler, P.A., Kless, S.J. & Adler, P. (1992) Socialization to gender
roles: Popularity among elementary school boys and girls. Sociology of
Education, 65, 169 – 187.
Brown, L.M. (2003) Girlfighting: Betrayal and rejection among girls.
New York: New York University Press.
Brown, L.M. & Gilligan, C. (1992) Meeting at the crossroads: Women’s
psychology and girls’ development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Bussey, K. & Bandura, A. (1984) Influence of gender constancy and
social power on sex-linked modeling. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 47, 1292-1302.
Cook, D.T. & Kaiser, S.B. (2004) Betwixt and between: Age ambiguity
and the sexualization of the female consuming subject. Journal of
Consumer Culture, 4, 203-227.
Friedrich, W.N., Fisher, J. L., Ditner, C.A., Acton, R., Berliner, L.
& Butler, J. (2001). Child Sexual Behavior Inventory: Normative,
psychiatric, and sexual abuse comparisons. Child Maltreatment, 6, 37-49.
York: Wiley.
Kendall-Tackett, K.A., Williams, L.M., & Finkelhor, D. (1993). Impact
of sexual abuse on children: A review and synthesis of recent empirical
studies. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 164-180.
Lacroix, C. (2004). Images of animated others: The Orientalization of
Disney’s cartoon heroines from the Little Mermaid to the Hunchback of
Notre Dame. Popular Communication, 2, 213-229.
Lamb, S. (2002) The secret lives of girls: What good girls really
do-Sex play, aggression, and their guilt. New York: Free Press.
Lamb, S. & Brown, L.M. (2006). Packaging girlhood: Rescuing our
daughters from marketers’ schemes. New York: St. Martin’s Press.
Letourneau, E.J., Schoenwald, S.K. & Sheidow, A.J. (2004). Children
and adolescents with sexual behavior problems. Child Maltreatment, 9,
49-61.
Levin, D.E. (2005). So sexy, so soon: The sexualization of childhood.
In S. Olfman (Ed). Childhood lost: How American culture is failing our
kids (pp. 137-153). Westport, CT:Praeger Press.
Martin, K.A. (1998). Becoming a gendered body: Practices in
preschools. American Sociological Review, 63, 494-511.
McHale, S.M., Crouter, A.C. & Tucker, C.J. (1999). Family context and
gender role socialization in middle childhood: Comparing girls to boys
and sisters to brothers. Child Development, 70, 990-1004.
Merskin, D. (2004). Reviving Lolita ? A media literacy examination of
sexual portrayals of girls in fashion advertising. American Behavioral
Scientist, 48, 119-129.
Merten, D.E. (2004). Securing her experience: Friendship versus
popularity. Feminism and Psychology, 14, 361-365.
Nelson, A. (2000). The pink dragon is female: Halloween costumes and
gender markers. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, 137-144.
O’Donohue, W., Gold, S.R. & McKay, J.S. (1997). Children as sexual
objects: Historical and gender trends in magazines. Sexual Abuse:
Journal of Research & Treatment, 9, 291-301.
Paik, H. (2001). The history of children’s use of electronic media.
In D.G. Singer & J.L. Singer (Eds.), Handbook of children and the media
(pp. 7-27). Thousand Oaks, CA:Sage.
Roberts, D., Foehr, U. & Rideout, V. (2005). Generation M: Media in
the lives of 8-18 year olds. Menio Park, C.A: Kaiser Family Foubndation.
Schor, J.B. (2004). Born to buy: The commercialized child and the new
consumer culture. New York: Scribner.
Sutton-Smith, B. (1986). Toys as culture. New York: Gardner Press.
Tenenbaum, H.R. & Leaper, C. (2002) Are parents’ gender schemas
related to their children’s gender-related congnition ? A meta-analysis.
Developmental Psychology, 38, 615-630.
Thompson, T. & Zerbinos, E. (1997). Cartoons: Do children notice it’s
a boy’s world ? Sex Roles, 37, 415-432.
Thorne, B. (1993). Gender play: Girls and boys in school. New
Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Villimez, C., Eisenberg, N. & Carroll, J.L. (1986). Sex differences
in the relation of children’s height and weight to academic performance
and others’ attributions of competence. Sex Roles, 15, 667-681.
Zurbriggen, E.L. & Freyd, J.J. (2004). The link between child sexual
abuse and risky sexual behavior: The role dissociative tendencies,
information-processing effects, and consensual sex decision mechanisms.
In L.J. Koenig, A. O’Leary, L.S. Doll, & W. Pequegnat (Eds.). From child
sexual abuse to adult sexual risk: Trauma revictimization and
intervention (pp. 117-134). Washinton, DC: American Psychological
Association.
APPENDIX B
References judged to be broadly relevant to infants and children
as mentioned under the heading “Consequences of the Sexualization of
Girls”:
Atkin, C. (1982). Television advertising and socialization of
consumer roles. In D. Pearl (Ed.). Television and behavior: Ten
years of scientific progress and implications for the eigthties (pp.
191-200). Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental Health.
Baron, A.S. & Banaji, M.R. (2006). The development of implicit
attitues: Evidence of race evaluations from ages 6 and 10 and adulthood.
Psychological Science, 17, 53-58.
Borzekowski, D.L.G. & Robinson, T.N. (1999). Viewing the viewers: 10
video case studies of children’s television viewing behaviors. Journal
of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 43, 506-528.
Brown, L.M. (1991). Telling a girl’s life: Self-authorization as a
form of resistance. In C. Gilligan, A.G. Rogers, & D.L. Tolman (Eds.),
Women, girls, and psychotherapy: Reframing resistance (pp. 71-86). New
York: Harrington Park Press.
Browne, A. & Finkelhor, D. (1986). Impact of child sexual abuse: A
review of the research. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 66-77.
Cook, D.T. & Kaiser, S.B. (2004) Betwixt and between: Age ambiguity
and the sexualization of the female consuming subject. Journal of
Consumer Culture, 4, 203-227.
Friedrich, W.N., Fisher, J.L., Ditner, C.A., Acton, R., Berliner, L.
& Butler, J. (2001). Child Sexual Behavior Inventory: Normative,
psychiatric, and sexual abuse comparisons. Child Maltreatment, 6, 37-49.
Greenberg, J. (2001). The ambiguity of seduction in the development
of Freuds’s thinking. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 37, 417-426.
Harter, S. (1998). The development of self-representations. In W.
Damon (Series Editor) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child
psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th
ed., pp. 553-617. Hoboken, N.J: Wiley.
Kendall-Tackett, K.A., Williams, L.M. & Finkelhor, D. (1993). Impact
of sexual abuse on children: A review and synthesis of recent empirical
studies. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 164-180.
Kindlon, D. & Thompson, M. (1999). Raising Cain: Protecting the
emotional life of boys. New York: Ballantine Books.
Lewis, M. (2000). Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment, pride,
shame, and guilt. In M. Lewis & J.M. Haviland-Jones (Eds). Handbook of
emotions (2nd ed., pp. 623-636). New York: Guilford Press.
Linn, S. (2005). The commercialization of childhood. In S. Olfman
(Ed.), Childhood lost: How American culture is failing our kids (pp.
107-121). Westport, CT: Praeger Press.
Merskin, D. (2004). Reviving Lolita? A media literacy examination of
sexual portrayals of girls in fashion advertising. American Behavioral
Scientist, 48, 119-129.
Nadon, S.M., Koverola, C. & Schludermann, E.H. (1998). Antecedents to
prostitution: Childhood victimization. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 13, 206-221.
O’Donohue, W., Gold, S.R. & McKay, J.S. (1997). Children as sexual
objects: Historical and gender trends in magazines. Sexual Abuse:
Journal of Research & Treatment, 9, 291-301.
Paul, B. (2004). Testing the effects of exposure to virtual child
pornography on viewer cognitions and attitudes toward deviant sexual
behavior. Dissertation Abstracts International, 64(8A), 2693A.
Polce-Lynch, M., Myers, B.J. & Kilmartin, C.T. (1998). Gender and age
patterns in emotional expression, body image and self-esteem: A
qualitative analysis. Sex Roles, 38, 1025-1048.
Pollack, W. (1998). Real boys: Rescuing our sons from the myths of
boyhood. New York: Random House.
President’s Council on Physical Fitness and Sports. (1997). Physical
activity and sport in the lives of girls: Executive summary. Washington,
DC:U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
Putnam, F.W. (1990). Disturbances of “self” in victims of childhood
sexual abuse. In R. Kluft (Ed.), Incest-related syndromes of adult
psychopathology (pp. 113-131). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Rachman, A.W.M., Kennedy, R. & Yard, M. (2005). The role of childhood
sexual seduction in the development of an erotic transference:
Perversion in the psychoanalytic situation. International Forum of
Psychoanalysis, 14, 183-187.
Roberton, M .A. & Halverson, I.E. (1984). Developing children: Their
changing movements. New York: Lea & Febiger.
Rutti, R. (1997, January 27). Putting gender into the equation:
Single sex classes seem to add up. The Plain Dealer, p. 1B.
Scharff, J.S. & Scharff, D. E. (1994). Object relations therapy of
physical and sexual trauma. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson.
Shengold, L. (1989). Soul murder: The effects of childhood abuse and
deprivation. New York: Ballantine Books.
Silbert, M.H. & Pines, A.M. (1981). Sexual child abuse as an
antecedent to prostitution. Child Abuse & Neglect, 5, 407-411.
Silbert, M.H. & Pines, A.M. (1982). Entrance into prostitution. Youth
and Society, 13, 471-500.
Simpkins, S.D., Davis-Kean, P.E. & Eccles, J.S. (2006). Math and
science motivation: A longitudinal examination of the links between
choices and beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 42, 70-83.
Smolak, L. & Murnen, S.K. (2002). A meta-analytic examination of the
relationship between child sexual abuse and eating disorders.
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31, 136-150.
Strasburger, V.C. & Wilson, B.J. (2002). Children, adolescents and
the media. Thousand Oaks, CA:Sage.
Warin, J. (2000). The attainment of self-consistency through gender
in young children. Sex Roles, 42,2009-231.
Young, I.M. (1980). Throwing like a girl: A phenomenology of feminine
body comportment motility and spatiality. Human Studies, 3, 137-156.
Another reference mentioned in the present paper and also copied
from the report is:
Lambiase, J. (2003). Sex–Online and in Internet advertising. In T.
Reichert & J. Lambiase (Eds). Sex in advertising: Perspectives on the
erotic appeal (pp. 247-269). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
A reference mentioned in the text of the report (page 30) but not
in its “References” is:
Moore, 2003.
* Arnold
Veraa is a former social worker and psychologist in child
protective services, Melbourne.
[Back] |