Giving up Cherished Ideas: The Rorschach Ink Blot Test1
Robyn M. Dawes*
One of the most dearly (expensively) held beliefs of many
clinical psychologists is the belief in the validity of Rorschach inkblot
interpretation. While this belief may be common in the general American
population, it is particularly strong among clinical psychologists, many of whom
still give Rorschachs despite the consistent research findings — of literally
thousands of published studies — that the Rorschach interpretation is unreliable
and invalid. The plausibility of Rorschach interpretation is so compelling that
it is still accepted in court proceedings involving involuntary commitment and
child custody, with psychologists who offer such interpretations in these
hearings being duly recognized as "experts."1
American Psychological Association rules of ethics prohibit
my presenting an example of a Rorschach inkblot. (Presumably, prior exposure to
these blots would contaminate the validity, if there were any, of any subsequent
use.) Suffice it to say that there are ten blots on cards roughly the size of
regular typing paper. Six of these are black and various shades of gray; the
remaining four have color. The blots themselves cover roughly half the area of
the cards on which they are reproduced, in a horizontal orientation — that is, the
position of a sheet of typing paper turned on its side. These blots are
symmetric around a vertical axis in the middle of the card. They were developed
by the psychiatrist Herman Rorschach (1884-1922), for purposes totally unrelated
to assessing character structure and personality problems.
The subject is asked to say what the cards look like to him
or her. The instructions are purposely vague, allowing subjects to make
associations from the form, shading, color, or texture of the blots. Moreover,
the subject can respond to each blot in its entirety, to major portions of the
blot ("large details"), or to small details in the blot's structure; subjects are also free to make use
of the white spaces surrounding the blot or within it. Finally, the subject is
free to rotate the cards from the positions in which they are presented — and even
to turn cards over and look at the back of them.
After the subject gives a response, the examiner asks him or
her to explain it with such questions as "Why does it look like a
bat?", "What in the blot makes it look like your grandfather drunkenly
falling off his chair at his fifty-fifth wedding anniversary celebration?",
or simply, "Tell me more." Moreover, subjects are urged to see more
than one percept per blot with queries such as "Anything else?"
The theory behind the test is simple. The world contains
ambiguity; people respond to the ambiguity in habitual ways, and the more
ambiguous the situation in which they find themselves the more important these
habitual response styles become. An inkblot, being the ultimate of ambiguity,
should be an ideal way to "tap into" such habitual responses. Moreover, the content that people see may give valuable clues about the types of
materials they "have on their mind" in that these are free to be projected into the stimulus situation, because it in fact has no structure of
its own. Hence, the Rorschach is termed a projective test. Moreover, as Freud
has suggested, the content of dreams and fantasies is particularly indicative of
our unconscious needs and conflicts, because there is no external stimulus to
which we are responding. By virtue of being a stimulus with minimal structure,
the Rorschach inkblots elicit projections of internally generated "percepts" —
which
can then be used to make inferences about unconscious needs and conflicts.
The theory is not only plausible but compelling. For example,
I recall testing a very depressed individual who immediately responded to a blot
by saying, "It looks like a bat that has been squashed on the pavement
under the heel of a giant's boot." What response could possibly be more
"one-down?" The fact that the individual was obviously depressed led
to my belief in the validity of the Rorschach. (Note that my observation can be
reframed to indicate that his response provided me with no information that I
did not already have.) Of course, if he had been obviously
psychotic, I could have noted that his percept concerned material not present in
the blot (e.g., the giant and the boot). That would have also impressed me,
because that response would indicate how he attended to stimuli not present in
the environment — virtually the definition of psychosis. Or, if he had suffered
from aggressive outbursts, I would have noted the hostility in the response.
I also recall testing a homosexual male nurse (at the time,
homosexuality was termed a "disease") who gave approximately forty
"vista" responses — for example, vistas of Chinese junks on lagoons with
mountains in the background. At the time, the prevailing theory about the
etiology of male homosexuality was that it was due to childhood withdrawal of
feelings from an overpowering mother who aroused incest fantasies and
identification with a weak, passive father — who had to be weak and passive or the
mother would not have been that way. How clearly these vista responses indicated
the man's pathological tendency to distance himself from emotionally threatening
material!
The compelling plausibility of Rorschach interpretation
should now be apparent. Clearly, for example, responding to the blots in their
entirety would seem to indicate a tendency to search for the "big
pictures" in life — even when they aren't there; motion responses must
indicate an active imagination; the use of white spaces, a tendency toward
oppositionality and perverseness, et cetera, et cetera. Moreover, seeing
something that the examiner cannot see must indicate very poor "reality
testing" — most probably psychosis. (At one staff meeting I attended, the head
psychologist successfully lobbied to have someone labeled
"schizophrenic" after waving a Rorschach blot in front of the group
and demanding, "Does this look like a bear to you?") Like the
unstructured interview, the Rorschach inkblot test is a major technique used by
many clinical psychologists.
In contrast to the compelling plausibility of the inkblot
test, what does the research show? Based on thousands of studies addressed to
this question, the answer is simple: damn little support for the projective
hypothesis. For example, one consistent finding is that the number of responses
the subject makes correlates with scores on intelligence tests. But then again
the amount a subject talks in any situation may have such a relationship, and
intelligence tests are better measures of intelligence than is the Rorschach
test.2
There are also certain intriguing findings, such as that
concerning the "index of existential pathology." This index refers to
the tendency to see part-human, part-nonhuman things — for example, cartoon characters, elves, satyrs, and witches.
One study indicated
that "neurotics" have a much higher tendency than do
"normals" or "eminent physical scientists" to see such
things.3 This finding appeared, however, at the end of a long paper on the
"psychodynamics of eminent physical scientists" in which none of the
other hypotheses tested was supported-for example, the hypothesis that eminent
physical scientists should have a greater tendency than others to refer to
"mother nature." Moreover, the scores on this index were trichonomized
in a post hoc manner into 0-3, 3-6, and 6 or more. Perhaps this categorization
was made to maximize the value of the statistic used to assess
"significance." Moreover, there is no mention in the literature of
replicating this difference. I mention this finding because it is typical of
intriguing findings involving the Rorschach. They appear and then disappear from
our body of knowledge.
What about the basic dimensions of personality and
psychopathology that the Rorschach purports to assess as a projective device?
Does it work? The answer to this question — at least up until 1978 —
may be found by
reading the reviews of the Rorschach in the Mental Measurement Yearbook.
First
published in 1938, the Yearbook was the work of Oscar K. Burros, who edited it
until his death. Beginning as a modest compilation of reviews of intelligence,
aptitude, interest, and personality tests, it became the major source of
information about all the tests in psychological literature — until its last
publication in 1978, which consisted of two volumes of roughly 1,000 pages each.
It was not truly a "yearbook," since it was published only every five
years or so. The Rorschach, and other projective tests, were first reviewed in
the third volume in 1949. There were two reviews, one favorable and one
unfavorable. The unfavorable one was by J. R. Wittenborn, who wrote (p.133):
What passes for research in this field is usually naively
conceived, inadequately controlled, and only rarely subjected to the usual
standards of experimental rigor with respect to the statistical tests and
freedom from ambiguity. Despite these limitations, the test flourishes, its
admirers multiply, and its claims proliferate.
The favorable review was by Morris Kruguman (p. 132):
The Rorschach withstood the clinical test well throughout
the years and has come out stronger for it; on the other hand, attempts at
atomistic validation have been unsuccessful and will probably continue to be
so.
Note that neither reviewer cited any studies that
demonstrated validity. The favorable one views these as "attempts at atomistic validation" —
which the
author derogates. (But what could be more "atomistic" than a
psychological diagnosis presented in a custody dispute? Such a diagnosis is a
qualitative characterization, and if attempts to validate such characterizations
on the basis of the Rorschach have been unsuccessful, how can they be made on
the basis of this test?) The unfavorable reviewer's characterization of the
research as shoddy left open the possibility that good research might show the
Rorschach to have some validity.
But if it does have validity, it is not reported in the next Yearbook. The
favorable review by Helen Sargent asserts instead (p.218) that
"the Rorschach test is a clinical technique, not a psychometric
method."
By the time the fifth Yearbook was published in 1959, the
world's leading expert on psychological testing, Lee Cronbach, is quoted in a
review: "The test has repeatedly failed as a prediction of practical
criteria. There is nothing in the literature to encourage reliance on Rorschach
interpretations."4 In addition, major reviewer Raymond
J. McCall writes
(p.154): "Though tens of thousands of Rorschach tests have been
administered by hundreds of trained professionals since that time (of a previous
review), and while many relationships to personality dynamics and behavior have
been hypothesized, the vast majority of these relationships have never been
validated empirically, despite the appearance of more than 2,000 publications
about the test." (Italics are in the original.) The other major reviewer,
Hans J. Eysenck, was even more negative. After presenting the Cronbach quote, he
reiterated again that there is absolutely no evidence for any of the claims of
the people using the Rorschach test.
In the sixth Mental Measurement Yearbook published in 1965,
Arthur R. Jensen wrote (p.509):
Many psychologists who have looked into the matter are
agreed that the 40 years of massive effort which have been lavished on the Rorschach
technique have proved unfruitful, at least so far as the development
of a useful psychological test is concerned.
And later,
The rate of scientific progress in clinical psychology
might well be measured by the speed and thoroughness with which it gets over
the Rorschach.
In the seventh Yearbook, John F. Knudsen, a professor of
clinical psychology and a practicing clinician, wrote (p. 440): "The
Rorschach has continued to be characterized by numerous scoring systems and an
overwhelming helming amount of negative research."
Finally, in the eighth Yearbook (1978), Richard H. Davis (p.
1045) concluded: "The general lack of predicted validity for the Rorschach
raises serious questions about its continued use in clinical practice."
Are there not other reviewers in these same volumes who
support the test? Yes, but none of them refer to any research results.
Instead,
they justify use of the Rorschach on the basis that it is a "very novel
interview," a "behavior sample," or "source" or that it
is a type of "structured interview" with which many clinical
psychologists have become comfortable. These claims somewhat vaguely reference
"experience"; in addition there are a few suggestions that appear once
and then disappear for using the Rorschach in a novel manner — such as having each
member of a distressed couple take the test separately and then requiring the
couple to reach a joint conclusion about what the blots look like. The problem,
of course, is that there is no evidence that this particular form of
"structured interview" is more effective than any other, and, as
pointed out elsewhere, only interviews structured to elicit certain specific
information are valid. Even if it were demonstrated that this type of
interviewing does provide valid information, there is still the question of
whether any of it has incremental validity — that is, whether it provides any
information that cannot be obtained from simpler and more reliable sources, such
as the history of past behavior. Why then does the Rorschach continue to be
used?
The answer may be found in the review of A. G. Bernstein in
the seventh Mental Measurement Yearbook in 1972. He wrote (p.434): "the
view that recognition, the act of construing an unfamiliar stimulus, taps
central components of personality functions is one that will remain crucial in
any psychology committed to the understanding of human experience." Despite his
misuse of the term recognition (which means noting that a stimulus has appeared
before in one's experience — the exact opposite of "construing an unfamiliar
stimulus"), I agree with Bernstein. He refers to a view, a plausible
assumption. If we adopt this assumption, the Rorschach should work.
The
overwhelming evidence that it does not work is ignored. Perhaps some other test
works, but this particular one fails. (One really interesting question is why
the same ten blots have been used for over fifty years, given the failure of the
technique and the simultaneous belief in the underlying "theory."
The
best hypothesis I can provide is that of "institutional inertia.")
What I believe is "crucial in any psychology," however, is not a
belief in the validity of the Rorschach, but an understanding of why people
believe it. The fascinating question is, who's projecting what and why?5
In contrast, a popular new
scoring system ("the Exner system") has empirical validity. The major
variables in this system that correlate with behavior, however, are based on
assessing the quality ("form level") of the responses. Such assessment is based on the assumption that parts of the
blots do resemble some shapes more than others, an assumption totally counter
to the "projective" one that it is the lack of structure in the blots
that leads to valid interpretations of subjects' responses.
Footnotes
1 Now that I am no longer a member of the American Psychological Association
Ethics Committee, I can express my personal opinion
that the use of Rorschach interpretations in establishing an individual's legal
status and child custody is the single most unethical practice of my colleagues.
It is done, widely. Losing legal rights as a result of responding to what is
presented as a "test of imagination ," often in a context
of "helping," violates what I believe to be a basic ethical principle
in this society — that people are judged on the basis of what they do, not on the
basis of what they feel, think, or might have a propensity to do. And being
judged on an invalid assessment of such thoughts, feelings, and propensities
amounts to losing one's civil rights on an essentially random basis. [Back]
2 A very eminent psychologist once proposed that
"intelligence is whatever it is that is measured by intelligence
tests." [Back]
3 McClelland, D. C. (1962). On the psychodynamics of creative
physical scientists. In Gruber, H.; Terrell, G.; and Wertheimer, M. (Eds.).
Contemporary Approaches to Creative Thinking. New York: Mather, 141-174. [Back]
4 Cronbach, L. J. (1958). Assessment of individual differences,
Annual Review of
Psychology, 7, Stanford, CA.: Annual Reviews, Inc., 448. [Back]
5 The materials in this section have been taken from the third
through the eighth Mental Measurement Yearbooks, all edited by Oscar K. Burros.
The third was published in New Brunswick, New Jersey, by the Rutgers University
Press; the fourth through the eighth were published in Highland Park, New Jersey,
by the Gryphon Press; the years were
1953, 1959, 1965, 1972, and 1978. [Back]
1 Excerpts from Rational Choice in an Uncertain World
()
by Robyn
M. Dawes, copyright © 1988 by Harcourt Brace
Jovanovich, Inc., reprinted by
permission of the publisher.
* Robyn M. Dawes is professor of Social and Decision Sciences
and Professor of Psychology at Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh,
Pennsylvania, 15290. [Back]
|