Anatomically-Detailed Dolls
Anatomically-detailed dolls are routinely used in interviewing children
suspected of being abused. The dolls are made of plastic or cotton and come
dressed with easily-removable clothing. There are several commercial manufacturers
who sell the dolls but they are also sometimes hand made. The mature female
dolls have representations of breasts that protrude and the boy and mature
male dolls have penises. There are holes in the dolls representing the mouth,
anus, and vagina. The penis is able to fit into these openings. Often, the
dolls have fingers that also fit into the openings. The mature male and
female dolls have pubic hair. Although some early versions of the dolls
appeared to have genitals that were disproportionately large, a survey of
17 sets of anatomical dolls (Bays, 1991) indicated that the genitals were
not exaggerated in size. The design of the dolls is not standardized (see
Figure 1).
Anatomical dolls are used by many different types of professionals (Boat
& Everson, 1988, 1996; Conte et al., 1991; Kendall-Tackett & Watson,
1992), many of whom may have little or no training in their use. Despite
their widespread use, these dolls are extremely controversial and there
is disagreement in the professional community as to whether they should
be used (e.g., Koocher et al., 1995; Yates & Terr, 1988). However, all
agree that they have no demonstrated validity and reliability.
The dolls are sometimes used in ways that can induce serious error into
the interview. In one videotape we reviewed, the mother, who was part of
the interview, took the dolls, put them in the intercourse position, and
then asked the child to do this. In another, the interviewer pointed to
and touched the genitals of a doll that had been labeled "daddy"
and told the child, "Show me where daddy touched you." Boat and
Everson (1996) describe an example of a young child putting the doll's penis
in her mouth but not responding when asked about it. Later, the interviewer
asked, "You put [the doll's] wienie in your mouth. Whose wienie have
you had in your mouth?" Boat and Everson (1996) also note that in 28%
of the interviews with 2- to 5-year-olds and 9% of the interviews with 6-
to 12-year-olds, the terms "play" or "pretend" were
used as part of the doll interview. To invite a young child to play or pretend
as a part of an interview about real events can be very confusing. This
is compounded if the adult then assumes the pretend behavior reflects actual
events.
We believe the dolls should not be used. There are no accepted standards
for their or normative data on them (APA Council of Representatives, 1991;
Koocher et al., 1995; Levy, Markovic, Kalinowski, Ahart, & Torres, 1995).
The dolls, in the way they are often used, may become learning experiences
for a child (Wakefield & Underwager, 1988; Underwager & Wakefield,
1990). Interviewers may model handling the dolls, undress them, or name
them for the child. They may ask the child to show with the dolls what the
accused person did. They may place the dolls in sexually explicit positions.
Although some researchers claim the dolls are not necessarily suggestive
(e.g., Everson & Boat, 1994), some nonabused children engage the dolls
in representations of sexual play (Dawson & Geddie, 1991; Dawson, Vaughan,
& Wagner, 1992; Everson & Boat, 1990; Gabriel, 1985; Glaser &
Collins, 1989; McIver, Wakefield, Underwager, 1989).
Studies that claim to show differences between the doll play of sexually
abused and nonabused children have major methodological shortcomings which
limit any conclusions that can be drawn from them (Ceci & Bruck, 1993;
Skinner & Berry 1993; Underwager & Wakefield, 1990, Wakefield &
Underwager, 1988, 1989, 1994; Wolfner, Faust, & Dawes, 1993). DeLoache
(1995b) notes that the basic reason for using anatomical dolls is the belief
that the dolls will elicit information from children who are unable or unwilling
to verbally describe the abuse. She observes, however, that not only is
there no good evidence that dolls help in interviews with young children,
but the presence of the dolls might result in the youngest children providing
less information. Younger children cannot understand the basic self-doll
relation assumed by interviewers. They cannot use dolls as symbols or representations
for themselves and therefore cannot use the dolls to enact their own experiences.
DeLoache concludes that the presence of an anatomical doll might even interfere
with the memory reports of younger children.
Wolfner et al. (1993) point out that the necessary research to determine
whether the dolls provide any incremental validity in establishing abuse
would involve a group of children who were all suspected of being
abused who, based on subsequent evidence, could be definitely divided into
those who have and have not been abused. The doll interviews would have
to take place prior to the children undergoing the standard procedures for
investigating sexual abuse, since the process of being questioned about
abuse could affect their reactions to the dolls. Such research has not been
done-the studies that are claimed to support the use of the dolls only compare
children suspected of abuse to those who are not suspected.
In summary, anatomical dolls are controversial, with some professionals
claiming they are useful and others contending that they are too suggestive.
Their use is especially problematical if the child's interaction with the
dolls forms the basis for a conclusion about sexual abuse. Some professionals
maintain that the dolls can be used if great care is taken not to be suggestive,
if the child's interaction with the dolls is not the basis for an opinion
about sexual abuse, and if they are not be used with very young children
(e.g., Boat & Everson, 1996; Koocher, et al., 1995; Simkins & Renier,
1996).
Others believe the dolls should not be used, even with great care (e.g.,
Fisher & Whiting, in press; Underwager & Wakefield, 1995; Wolfner
et al., 1993). They are unnecessary for older children and risk introducing
error into the accounts of younger children. There is no empirical evidence
that doll interviews are a valid and reliable method for getting accurate
information. The use of the dolls as an assessment or investigatory technique
is not generally accepted within the scientific community, rather, their
use remains highly controversial.