Interviews of Children
During the 1980s there was a major change in professional opinions concerning
the susceptibility of children to suggestive and leading interviews. The
first time a psychologist testified in a courtroom as an expert was 1896
when Schrenk-Notzing testified in Munich that witnesses were influenced
by suggestion to produce "retroactive memory-falsification" (Bartol
and Bartol, 1987). From then until fairly recently this was the prevailing
view of children's vulnerability to suggestion. Then a shift occurred. The
testimony of young children was generally accepted as truthful and the prevailing
opinion was that young children could not be led or "coached"
to make statements about abuse that never happened. The belief was that,
although children might be led through suggestive interviews to make unimportant
errors concerning peripheral details, they could not be led to make statements
about important, central events.
But, as researchers became involved in actual cases and reviewed videotapes
of actual interviews, they observed that the research supporting the above
claims did not begin to duplicate what often happens in the real world.
As a result, there has been new research that has changed the consensus
of scientific opinion. A number of writers have examined memory development,
cognitive and moral development of children, and suggestibility of children
to adult social influence (see Doris, 1991; Garbarino and Stott, 1989; Lepore,
1991; Lindsay, 1990; Loftus and Ketcham, 1991; Underwager and Wakefield,
1990; Wakefield and Underwager, 1988a, 1994c). The best summary of this
research is in articles by Steve Ceci and Maggie Bruck and in an Amicus
Curiae brief they presented to the New Jersey Supreme Court in New
Jersey vs. Michaels (Bruck and Ceci, 1994; Ceci, 1994; Ceci and Bruck,
1993a, 1993b).
The fact that children can be led to make statements about and even believe
in events that have not happened does not necessarily mean that children
lie, but rather that they are influenced by the adult's beliefs. Some recent
studies have provided dramatic demonstrations of the degree to which young
children can be influenced by an interviewer (Ceci, 1994; Ceci and Bruck,
1993a, 1993b; Ceci et al., 1994; Clarke-Stewart et al., 1989; Leichtman
and Ceci, in press; Thompson et al., 1991).
In situations where a child will eventually testify, the memory will consist
of a combination of recall and reconstruction influenced by all of the interviews,
conversations, and therapy sessions that have occurred during the delay.
The longer the delay, the greater the possibility of social influence and
the more the memory may consist of reconstruction rather than recall.
In a careful and thorough review, Ceci and Bruck (1993a) state there are
several conclusions that are accepted by the scientific community and would
meet "a traditional Frye test standard" (p. 431). Attorneys
must be familiar with this article and any mental health professional who
makes contrary claims should be questioned concerning it. Any mental health
professional who does not know and understand the relevant research in this
area is extremely vulnerable to cross-examination by a knowledgeable attorney.
Ceci and Bruck's (1993a) major three conclusions are:
First and foremost, contrary to the claims made by some...there do appear
to be significant age differences in suggestibility, with preschool-aged
children being disproportionately more vulnerable to suggestion than either
school-aged children or adults. (p. 431)
Ceci and Bruck also observe that the literature does not support the claim
that children are not suggestible concerning central events:
Our review of the literature indicates that children can indeed be led to
make false or inaccurate reports about very crucial, personally experienced,
central events. (p. 432)
Ceci and Bruck's next major conclusion is:
The second major conclusion is that contrary to the claims of some, children
sometimes lie when the motivational structure is tilted toward lying. (p.
433)
Finally, they state:
Third, notwithstanding the aforementioned two points, it is clear that children-even
preschoolers-are capable of recalling much that is forensically relevant.
(p. 433)
They add that is is extremely important to examine the conditions prevalent
at the time of the child's original report:
If the child's disclosure was made in a nonthreatening, nonsuggestible atmosphere,
if the disclosure was not made after repeated interviews, if the adults
who had access to the child prior to his or her testimony are not motivated
to distort the child's recollections through relentless and potent suggestions
and outright coaching, and if the child's original report remains highly
consistent over a period of time, then the young child would be judged to
be capable of providing much that is forensically relevant. The absence
of any of these conditions would not in and of itself invalidate a child's
testimony, but it ought to raise cautions in the mind of the court. (p.
433)
The important point is that, although young children can provide forensically
useful information, the major problem is that adults do not know how to
let them do it (Garbarino and Stott, 1989). Young children recall less than
adults (Lepore, 1991). But the less information the child gives in free
recall, the sooner the interviewer may start using leading questions, which
can influence the child and distort the story. Also, young children may
perceive the interview task differently from adults and try to tell the
interviewer what they believe the interviewer wants them to say (Ceci et
al., 1987; Cole and Loftus, 1987). They may answer questions they do not
understand and about which they have no information (Hughes and Grieve,
1983).
Therefore, the interviewer must encourage the child to tell in his or her
own words what has happened. Several professionals have made recommendations
for conducting an unbiased evaluation and noncontaminating interview (e.g. Annon, 1994; Daly, 1991, 1992a, 1992b; Powell and Thomson, 1994; Quinn et
al., 1989; Raskin and Yuille, 1989; Slicner and Hanson, 1989; Wakefield
and Underwager, 1988a; 1994c; White, 1990; Yuille, 1988). There is general
agreement among scientists as to how investigatory interviews should be
conducted. The interviewer should ask open-ended questions and encourage
the child to provide a free narrative. Details should be encouraged by responses
such as "and then what happened." Pressure and coercion, leading
questions and selective reinforcement of responses, and unvalidated techniques
must be avoided. The child should be discouraged from trying to answer questions
when the answer is not known. Discussions of "good touch" and
"bad touch" should not be used since these are confusing and potentially
contaminating.
Repeated questions should be avoided since this tells the child the previous
answers were not acceptable. The effects of leading questions and coercive
interviews will be worse with a long time lag, which is typical in actual
cases. Poole and White (1991, 1993) found that young children were accurate
if repeated but appropriate (open-ended) questioning was used immediately
or one week after the event. But when questioned again two years later,
repeated questioning increased inaccuracy. The children often seemed to
simply make up responses and the authors conclude that, although children
can be prompted to discuss a remote event, this procedure is not without
risks. It cannot be assumed that results from research studies using short
retention intervals can be generalized to actual cases which often have
long delays.
A technique for interviewing children and analyzing the resulting interview
is Criterion Based Content Analysis/Statement Validity Analysis (CBCA/SVA).
This technique assumes an account based on memory for an actual event will
differ in content and quality from accounts that are based on fabricated,
learned, or suggested memory. The procedure requires a relatively complete
statement obtained as soon as possible after the child has disclosed an
incident and the interview must be designed to obtain as much free narrative
as possible. Leading questions and suggestions must be avoided. The interview
is tape-recorded and transcribed for later analysis (Honts, 1994, Köhnken
and Steller, 1988; Raskin and Esplin, 1991a; Undeutsch, 1989). There is
research on this technique, but most professionals encountered will not
have used it and few interviews in actual cases meet the above standards.
A leading, suggestive interview cannot be analyzed with this technique.
Also, if the use of this technique is encountered, the attorney will want
to be familiar with observations about and criticisms of the technique.
Wells and Loftus (1991) are concerned about the adequacy of the empirical
support for CBCA/SVA, the ability of the technique to differentiate individual
and age-related differences in linguistic abilities from validity-related
differences, and the potential for judges and juries to give the technique
too much weight. McGough (1991) observes that many of the criteria are found
in standard texts on cross-examination and closing arguments on witness
credibility. Since an expert using this technique would therefore be testifying
about the truthfulness of the statement, the reliability of the expert would
then become an issue. Raskin and Esplin (1991b) defend the empirical basis
of the technique and state that they do not advocate the use of CBCA as
the basis of expert testimony that a child is or is not truthful. They believe
that expert testimony based on CBCA is consistent with both the rules of
evidence and the growing body of case law regarding expert testimony by
psychologists.
All interviews of the child should be videotaped, or at least audiotaped,
since a tape is the only means whereby the procedures and information obtained
during the interview can be accurately documented (DeLipsey and James, 1988;
Goodman and Helgeson, 1985; Herbert et al., 1987; Jenkins and Howell, 1994;
Lamb, 1994a, 1994b; Raskin and Yuille, 1989; Underwager and Wakefield, 1990,
Wakefield and Underwager, 1988a, 1994c). A major research project on child
victim witnesses reported by Myers (1994) found a clear consensus that investigative
interviews of children should be videotaped. Videotaping was seen as providing
an incentive for interviewers to use proper techniques.
In practice, this is often not done. Many prosecutors do not want the defense
to get a tape so that they can criticize the interviewer's techniques during
the trial (Stern, 1992). We believe that there are no good reasons for failing
to at least audiotape investigatory interviews with the child witness; there
are only bad reasons. Without a tape, there is no way to know what was said
by either the child or the interviewer. When cross-examining a professional
who has not taped the interview, assume that the interview was leading
and suggestive, and do not accept assertions that it was not.
Despite the fact that the standards for investigatory interviews are accepted
in the scientific community, these guidelines are often not followed by
people in the field. Deficiencies in interviews and evaluations can be pointed
out in trial. Judges and juries can readily understand why such interviews
are unreliable once the problems are explained to them; we have frequently
testified concerning appropriate interviewing techniques and contrasting
these with those in a particular case. Such testimony, since it is about
the interview techniques and not about the credibility of the child, does
not invade the province of the finder-of-fact. Attorneys, once they become
familiar with how interviews should be conducted, can bring out the problems
in a particular interview or investigation in their cross-examinations of
the professionals involved.
The progress of the case, including the procedures followed by the interviewers
and evaluators, must be carefully examined (Wakefield and Underwager, 1988a;
White and Quinn, 1988). It is essential to analyze all contacts with the
child in which abuse was discussed. When children have been subjected to
multiple leading and coercive interviews and/or disclosure-based therapy
their recollections may become so contaminated that it becomes extremely
difficult to determine what is likely to have happened. The New Jersey Supreme
Court in New Jersey vs. Michaels (642 A.2d 1372, N.J. 1994) states:
[1] We therefore determine that a sufficient consensus exists within the
academic, professional, and law enforcement communities, confirmed in varying
degrees by courts, to warrant the conclusion that the use of coercive or
highly suggestive interrogation techniques can create a significant risk
that the interrogation itself will distort the child's recollection of events,
thereby undermining the reliability of the statements and subsequent testimony
concerning such events. (p. 1379)
Heuristics are specific mental strategies, rules, or short cuts that allow
us to solve specific problems. An example of a heuristic is the assumption
that a low-number license plate is associated with power or wealth. Although
we all use heuristics regularly, they are often without empirical support
or justification and may be completely wrong. Some of the heuristics uncovered
in decision theory research that are likely operative in the investigation
and interviewing process may be underutilization of base rates, the confirmatory
bias, the selective recall of illusory correlations, the availability fallacy,
and the representativeness fallacy. Confusions of correlation and causation
may also occur (Arkes, 1989).