Female Child Sexual Abusers: A Critical Review of the Literature
Hollida Wakefield, M.A. and Ralph Underwager, Ph.D.*
Awareness about female sexual abuse perpetrators has increased in
recent years. There is a great range in the estimated frequently
from different studies and the definition of sexual abuse, sample
selected, and methodology must be considered. Taken as a whole, the
literature indicates that although most sexual abusers are males, child
sexual abuse by females does occur and may be less rare than was once
believed. There are widely different circumstances under which women
sexually abuse children and these circumstances may often differ from
those causing men to do so. Many studies depict female abusers as
socially isolated, loners, alienated, coming from abusive backgrounds and
having emotional problems, although most are not psychotic. However,
some of the recent literature is likely to have included cases of false
accusations which gives a misleading picture of the frequency of female
sexual abuse and the characteristics of such women.
Most sexual offenders are men. Men commit
most of the aberrant and deviant sexual behaviors such as rape, child molestation
and exhibitionism. More males have paraphilias such as frotteurism,
voyeurism. fetishism, coprophilia, necrophilia, and sexual sadism.
There are more male transsexuals and transvestites. The DSM-III-R (1)
reports that except for sexual masochism, in which the sex ratio is
estimated to be 20 males for each female, the other paraphilias are
practically never diagnosed in females, although some cases have been
reported.
Therefore, until recently, women have not been
viewed as sexual abuse perpetrators except in unusual circumstances.
Women who did sexually abuse children were considered seriously
disturbed. Maternal incest in particular has been believed to be
extremely rare (2-5).
However, currently there is increased interest in women as perpetrators
of child sexual abuse and some researchers suggest it is more common than
previously believed. But there is still considerable disagreement
and confusion about just how often women sexually abuse children, what
type of woman is a sexual abuser, and under what circumstances the abuse
occurs.
METHODOLOGICAL PROBLEMS
Definitions of Sexual Abuse
The differences in rates of abuse by females and the characteristics of
such women reported in the literature are likely to be due to the
different definitional and methodological framing of the research
questions along with the nature of the sample used. Therefore, it is
not surprising that the studies report such different results.
Finkelhor and Russell (6)
note that some studies contain definitional problems which inflate the
statistics concerning female perpetrators. For example, the National
Incidence Study figures suggest that almost half of the sexual experiences
of children included a female perpetrator. However, according to the
study definitions, a caretaker could be a perpetrator if she
"permitted acts of sexual contact to occur." Therefore, if
a mother neglected a child while a father sexually abused the child, the
mother would be listed as a sexual abuse perpetrator. Also, a mother
could be listed as an active perpetrator if she did not adequately
supervise the child's voluntary sexual activities. The study by the
American Humane Association had similar problems. When the data were
reanalyzed to exclude these types of cases, the figures suggested that 14
percent of perpetrators against boys and six percent of perpetrators
against girls were females acting alone.
Travin, Cullin and Protter (7)
observe that the concepts sexual abuse and sexual offense are often
confused. Although these terms are often used interchangeably,
sexual abuse involves a sexual act perpetrated against an individual
without consent, whereas sexual offense describes the same behavior but
denotes it as a criminal act.
There are significant variations in definitions of sexual abuse in the
literature. The definitions vary in terms of their criteria and in
their specificity. They differ in the inclusion of noncontact along
with contact. Some specify an upper age limit along with a minimum
age discrepancy while others do not. Some rely on the respondent's
perception of whether the event was wanted or unwanted.
Is it sexual abuse when a child catches a glimpse of an
exhibitionist? There is disagreement over this. Most people
would not consider it abuse if a child is shown a Playboy magazine by an
older playmate. But what if a young child is shown hard core
pornography? Does a mother sleeping with her child constitute sexual
abuse in the absence of sexual touching? What if the child is a
teenager who becomes aroused by this? Most people would agree that
it is sexual abuse if a 19-year-old woman has sexual contact with a
6-year-old boy, but not if the boy is 16. However, what if the boy
is 14? What if the 14-year-old boy initiates the experience with the
woman and later views the experience as positive? Some of the
retrospective surveys of childhood sexual experiences include reports of
sexual contacts with older women which were perceived as positive by the
respondents and were initiated by the respondents. How such issues
are handled will effect the information obtained.
Retrospective Surveys
Retrospective studies are a common source of
information about the prevalence of sexual contact between women and
children. Such studies therefore suffer from the difficulties of all
such retrospective data. Finkelhor (8)
notes that "it is well-established in survey research that the
validity of reports declines with the distance from the event."
Memory is basically a reconstructive process, and what is recalled depends
upon our current beliefs and feelings (9-11).
We literally "make up stories" about our lives and reality (9)
and may even come to believe in memories of events that never happened (11).
Surveys using retrospective reports do not have external verification of
the information provided by the respondents.
Raphael, Cloitre and Dohrenwend (12)
report a study comparing ten months of concurrent monthly recall with a
final retrospective recall using event checklists. The level of
concordance was so low they conclude "... the results are devastating
for the accuracy of reporting event categories." Gerlsma,
Emmelkamp and Arrindell (13), in
their meta-analysis of parental rearing styles, comment on the dangers in
retrospective data and discuss false accounts and fabricated
accounts. Green and Hall (14)
in discussing quantitative methods and dependent variables describe
retrospective self-reports as especially tenuous.
It is generally assumed that this difficulty will
result in underestimates of the actual rate of sexual abuse because people
will have repressed their memories or will be hesitant to talk about
them. But retrospective data can also result in overestimation if
some individuals, as a result of the media attention to sexual abuse or
misguided psychotherapy, become convinced they have been sexually
abused. Ross (15) suggests
that a person may come to attribute to the parents those behaviors now
believed to be the cause of the individual's current state and generate a
revisionist history to justify the present.
The accuracy of what is reported in a
questionnaire to an interviewer will depend upon the way the questions are
worded, the manner by which the interview is administered, and the skills
of the interviewer. The data will be influenced by the population
sampled, the sampling techniques, and the response rates of those in the
sample (16).
For example, Okami (17)
observes that Russell (18), in her
survey of incest history in women, carefully selected and trained her
interviewers to be sympathetic to victims, to know how to ask questions to
encourage disclosure, and to disbelieve "myths" such as a belief
that sexual contact between an adult and child could ever be seen as
benign. Okami states that this ideological approach distorted the
results and this is why Russell reported prevalence rates for incest that
have greatly exceeded those in other studies.
Okami (17) also describes Finkelhor's
(17a) instructions in his 1979
survey of childhood sexual experiences in college students: "Some of
these (childhood sexual experiences) are very upsetting and painful and
some are not" and notes that this sets the stage for negative
reports. He wonders what Finkelhor's reaction would be if some other
investigator had used the instructions: "Some of these experiences
are very delightful and pleasurable and some are not." Also,
when the subject marked an experience as neutral, the designation was
graded by coders as negative if there was an age discrepancy of more than
five years. Okami gives several other examples to illustrate his
assertion that much of the current victimology-based research employs
polemical devices and research methods that blur the line between social
science and social criticism. Because of this, he maintains,
empirical truths are ignored or distorted in the interests of furthering
the cause.
Sample Characteristics
The major difficulty with studies based upon
specialized samples is that they are not representative of cases which did
not come to the attention of authorities or where the adult did not seek
help from a mental health professional. Many of the retrospective
surveys use college students. However, college students are unlike
noncollege students in many ways and information from this population may
not be representative of other groups (19).
Several studies use prison populations, both for
studies of adults who were perpetrators of sexual abuse and for adults who
were childhood victims. Finkelhor and Russell (6)
note that most sexual offenders are never reported and the number of those
who are caught and convicted and end up in treatment is even
smaller. This group, therefore, is unlikely to be representative of
sex offenders in general. Also, prisoners who report childhood
sexual molestation are atypical in several respects, including
socioeconomic status, education and sociopathy. In addition, there
is evidence that convicted sexual offenders in treatment are likely to
report childhood sexual abuse when it may have never happened (20).
Clinical samples also present problems. Any
generalization of dynamics and characteristics based on a clinician's
experience is limited to victims or perpetrators who have sought
therapy. In addition, clinical experience and clinical impressions
form a notoriously unreliable base for drawing conclusions (21,
22). Therefore, although clinical
and case studies provide a beginning point for developing hypotheses and
therefore contribute to the knowledge in the field, their limitations must
be kept in mind.
The Finkelhor Sexual Abuse In Day Care Study
David Finkelhor and his colleagues (23,
24), in a national study of 270 day care
cases, report that 40 percent of the perpetrators were women. These
women tended to be intelligent, educated, highly regarded in their
communities, and not likely to have a history of known deviant
behavior. Many of these apparently normal women were alleged to have
engaged in extremely deviant, low frequency behavior, including
oral-genital penetration, urolagia and coprophagia, and ritualistic, mass
abuse.
This study has received both popular and
professional attention and it is likely that it will be cited in the
future as evidence that apparently ordinary women are sexually abusing
children. Finkelhor and his colleagues recommend that parents and
licensing and law enforcement officials be educated to view females as
potential sexual abusers. Coleman (25)
gives an example of how, shortly after its publication, the study was
cited in a California Grand Jury as support for vigorous prosecution of
such cases.
There are significant difficulties with the methodology of this
study. Although the authors required the abuse to be
"substantiated," their definition of substantiation was whether
any one of the individuals assigned to investigate the report believed
that abuse was real, despite whoever else may have thought it was
false. They say "our way of defining substantiation is only a
way of approximating the truth ... Whenever we refer to cases, the
reader should not automatically assume that we, or anybody else, knows
with absolute certainty that these are cases of abuse rather than mistaken
allegations" (23).
Therefore, their sample includes an indeterminate number of cases which
ended in dismissals or acquittals, or convictions that were later
reversed. For example, the McMartin case, which later ended in
dropped charges and acquittals, is included.
Descriptions of ritualistic abuse are presented as
fact in spite of the c6mplete lack of any corroborating evidence for such
allegations (26). Finkelhor
et al. remark that the abusers, 40 percent of whom were women, did not fit
any stereotypes about sexual abusers. They note the
"disturbing" fact that some parents "failed to believe
their own children's allegations" and claim because very few staff
members were the source of disclosures, there was sometimes an actual
coverup of the abuse (24).
No conclusions whatsoever can be drawn about the characteristics of
perpetrators from studies with such a flawed and questionable criterion
measure.
The Finkelhor et al. study is the most obvious example of the problem
of cases of false accusations included in the sample. In defending their
choice the authors claim there is no reason to believe investigators err
on one side or the other (23).
Given the extensive theoretical discussion and empirical research on the
cognitive activity of the clinician, the research on the reliability of
diagnosis, the wide and broad research on decision theory, the more than
forty years of research on clinical versus statistical decisions with not
a single study supporting confidence in clinical observations and
judgment, and the failure of research to demonstrate any efficacy to
clinical experience, this is a surprising claim (9,
21, 22, 27-30).
It can only be the result of ignorance or a deliberate choice to obscure
and obfuscate the issues.
In other instances researchers may have inadvertently included falsely
accused persons in their sample. Therefore, the possibility of women
wrongfully convicted or falsely accused must always be considered when
reviewing the recent literature on female perpetrators.
Faller (31)
reports on a clinical sample of 87 boy victims and 226 girl victims of
validated sexual abuse. The female victims were more likely than
male victims to be sexually abused by a man. Neither boys nor girls
were very likely to be victimized by a woman alone, but this happened with
boys ten times more often than with girls. Of the 87 boys, 55 (63.1
percent) were victimized by a man alone, seven (8 percent) were victimized
by a woman alone, and 25 (28.7 percent) were victimized by both females
and males together. Faller reports that the largest category of
offenders was professionals, including day care workers.
In this study, the validation criteria was primarily the child's
statements and a clinical interview, although other evidence such as
perpetrator confession, witnesses and medical findings were also relied
upon when present. Therefore, this sample may include an unknown
number of false accusations. The fact that Faller reports the
largest category of offenders was professionals, including day care
workers, suggests this may have similar difficulties to the Finkelhor day
care study.
FREQUENCY OF SEXUAL ABUSE BY FEMALES
Three retrospective survey studies of college
students found large proportions of female perpetrators reported by males
who acknowledged a history of childhood sexual abuse. Fritz, Stoll
and Wagner (32) administered a
questionnaire to 952 male and female college students regarding sexual
abuse when they were children and found that 4.8 percent of the males
reported they had been molested. Of these, 60 percent were molested
by females, primarily older female adolescents.
Risin and Koss (33)
surveyed 2972 male college students and found that 216 (7.3 percent) met
one of their three criteria for sexual abuse (age discrepancy between
child and perpetrator, use of coercion, or perpetrator who was a care
giver or authority figure). Their definition was very broad, and
included some consensual activities with adolescent females. The
abusive behaviors ranged from exhibition to penetration. They report
that there were almost as many female perpetrators (42.7 percent) as male
(53.3 percent), with a small proportion involving both a male and a female
together (4.2 percent). Almost half of the female perpetrators were
adolescent babysitters. Almost half of the boys involved with female
perpetrators reported that they participated in the incidents voluntarily
and did not feel victimized. The authors note that this suggests
qualitatively different experiences were tapped in this study compared to
other surveys.
Fromuth and Burkhart (34)
surveyed 582 men from two colleges and found that, depending upon the
definition of childhood sexual abuse, prevalence rates varied from 4
percent to 24 percent being defined as abused. The majority (78
percent and 72 percent in the two samples) of the perpetrators of sexual
abuse were females. They also found that, compared to women in
college survey studies, men are less likely to perceive childhood sexual
experiences as abusive, which is consistent with the Risen and Koss (33)
survey. It may be that women perceive such experiences as sexual
violation, while men perceive them as sexual initiation. Male
socialization encourages men to define sexual experiences as desirable as
long as there is no homosexual involvement. The authors emphasize
the need to consider how sexual abuse is defined, particularly in studies
of male victims.
Finkelhor, on the other hand, found much smaller proportions of female
perpetrators in his student samples. Only 6 percent of college women
and 16 percent of college males who reported childhood abuse indicated
that the perpetrator was a woman. His survey using a community
sample of residents from Boston yielded similar figures: none of the women
and 15 percent of men reporting childhood abuse indicated that a woman was
the perpetrator (6).
A small percentage of female perpetrators was also
found by Russell (18) who states
that her sample of 930 women only reported ten cases of incestuous abuse
by females. The perpetrators included a biological mother, three
sisters, three first cousins and three more distant relatives. These
ten relatives constituted only five percent of all incest perpetrators and
affected only one percent of the 930 women interviewed. The
percentage of female perpetrators of extrafamilial child sexual abuse was
four percent. Russell's survey provides no information on the
frequency with which females abuse males.
The necessity of considering differences in the
type of sample and research method is demonstrated by contrasting the
above studies to those reported by Reinhart (35),
who found that only four percent of 189 boy victims were alleged to be
abused by females and Farber et al. (36),
who reported that only two percent of 162 children (half boys and half
girls) were sexually abused by a female acting alone (six percent were
abused by both a male and a female). Both Reinhart and Farber et al.
used clinical as opposed to college samples and their studies were of
current cases in a hospital as opposed to a retrospective survey.
Neither study is clear as to how the abuse was substantiated.
A still smaller percentage of female perpetrators
was found by Rowan, Rowan and Langelier (37),
who report that only nine (1.5 percent) of 600 sex offenders referred for
evaluations were females. This study, however, was on sex offenders
rather than on victims. The previous studies focused on victims.
The necessity of specifying the sample and
methodology is also shown by contrasting the Fromuth and Burkhart (34)
and the Risen and Koss (33)
retrospective surveys of college males to a report by Johnson and Shrier (38)
on eleven cases of molestation in a community based sample of adolescent
male outpatients. Eight of these boys experienced the molestation as
intensely traumatic. The female molesters were usually acquaintances
of the victims most often a neighbor, babysitter, or other trusted
older adolescent or young adult. All but one of the female molesters
used persuasion rather than physical force or threats and three-quarters
of the female molesters attempted to get their victims to ejaculate, and
nearly half succeeded. Johnson and Shrier therefore conclude that
childhood sexual victimization of boys by women as well as men is a high
risk and traumatic experience. In contrast, the two college surveys
indicate that many men did not feel victimized by the experience.
Studies using prison samples show high percentages
of men reporting childhood sexual experiences with older women.
Groth (6) found that 51 percent of a
sample of sex offenders had been molested when they were young and of
these, 25 percent had been by a female. Petrovich and Templer (39)
found that 59 percent of 83 convicted rapists reported heterosexual
experiences before the age of 16 with a female at least five years
older. The authors stated that they did not know whether this high
rate was a function of low socioeconomic status, being rapists, being sex
criminals more generally, or being criminals in general.
Condy, Templer, Brown and Veaco (40)
surveyed 359 male college students and 212 male prison inmates concerning
childhood heterosexual contact. Of the male prisoners, 46 percent
reported early sexual contact. This differed by offense with 57
percent of the rapists, 37 percent of the child molesters and 47 percent
of the nonsexual offenders reporting such contact. In contrast, only
16 percent of the college students reported childhood sexual contact with
a woman. Both the prisoners and college men reported more good
feelings than bad at the time of the incident and, with the exception of
the child molesters, the experience was regarded as having more of a good
than a bad effect on their adult sex lives.
In every category for the subjects who reported sexual contact with a
female, intercourse was involved in at least half of the sexual
encounters. In only a minority of the cases did the female force the
boy. In fact, in a large proportion of the cases, the male reported
initiating the activity. The distribution for the ages at which the
boys first became involved was skewed toward the higher ages; the median
age was 13 and the mode was 15. Condy et al. speculate that the
nature of these contacts could be more characterized by an extension of
the lower end of the age distribution for adult male-female sexual
relationships, in contrast to what ordinarily occurs when the molester is
a male. Also, although prepubescent vaginal size would make penile
penetration of a girl by an adult male difficult, the prepubescent penile
size would actually be less difficult with a young boy and a woman.
These higher percentages reported in prison populations could be due to
many factors, such as lower socioeconomic status and higher
sociopathy. However, it could also be due to a possible tendency, as
discussed earlier, for a prisoner to report being sexually abused when
this has actually not happened. At any rate, data from prison
populations cannot be generalized to the population at large.
Bolton, Morris and MacEachron (41)
conclude that male perpetrators far exceed female perpetrators of child
sexual abuse. They observe, however, that the interest in studying
female offenders has increased markedly in the past few years and note
that no matter how the differing rates found in the various studies are
explained, the fact remains that females are sometimes perpetrators.
HYPOTHESIZED BARRIERS TO RECOGNITION
Finkelhor and Russell (6) note
that despite ample opportunities for sexual abuse, remarkably few mothers
seem to take advantage of them. They conclude that the literature
"leads fairly persuasively to the conclusion that the traditional
view about child molestation as a primarily male deviation is essentially
correct. Women do not use children for their own direct sexual
gratification very frequently." However, other researchers have
challenged this and asserted that barriers have prevented the recognition
of female child molestation.
Allen (42)
claims that women may be perpetrators more often than is realized and
believes that there are several barriers preventing the recognition of
female child sexual abuse. The first barrier suggested is an
overestimation of the strength of the incest taboo, in which the taboo was
thought to be so strong that sexual abuse by women was an extremely rare
aberration. When it occurred, it was seen as evidence that the woman
was seriously impaired.
A possible second barrier to recognition has been an overextension of
feminist explanations of child sexual abuse. Here, child sexual
abuse is considered to be a direct result of culturally-based
socialization which leads to male dominance and subsequent exploitation of
women and children.
The third proposed barrier is the
overgeneralization of the empirical observation that female perpetrated
child sexual abuse is rare. That is, the frequency of reports in the
literature may not accurately reflect the frequency of actual
occurrence. Allen notes that even if there are far fewer female than
male perpetrators, there may still be a significant number of females who
abuse children. Allen concludes by recommending the strategy
suggested by Bolton and Bolton (43):
"Awareness of female sexual abuse perpetration is increasing
... It seems wise to withhold judgment about such cases until more
is known."
Groth (44)
suggests three possible reasons for the less frequent identification of
female sexual abuse perpetrators: I) women may mask sexually inappropriate
contact through activities such as bathing or dressing the child; 2) the
sexual offenses of women are more incestuous in nature and therefore
children are more reluctant to disclose the abuse when the offender is a
parent and someone they depend on; and, 3) boys may be more frequent
targets of abuse by females than are girls, but it may be difficult to
confirm this since boys are less likely to disclose abuse.
Justice and Justice (18) suggest
that mother-son incest is seldom reported in the literature because
mothers engage in types of sexual activity that does not get
reported. Examples of such activities are sleeping with a son,
fondling and caressing him in a sexual way, exposing her body to him, and
keeping him tied to her emotionally with promises of a sexual payoff.
Banning (45)
also maintains the incidence of child sexual abuse by women is
underestimated. She notes that feminists view child sexual abuse as
a crime committed against girls by men and until recently mother-child
incest was considered to be virtually nonexistent. She claims female
sexual abuse is not recognized because of the disbelief that this can
occur. In addition, women are permitted a freer range of sexual
contact with their children through caregiving activities and sexually
abusive behavior may be more difficult to recognize. Although more
recent studies have shown a higher incidence of female perpetrators, there
has been little research on them and their psychopathology may be
different from that of male perpetrators. Banning believes, however,
that the incidence of female sexual offenders will probably remain much
lower than that of males.
Sax (46)
believes the abusive nature of erotic and sexual encounters between women
and children is exaggerated. She notes that the relationship between
women and children is such that nobody ever thinks about sexual or erotic
involvement. However, such relationships do exist and not all such
encounters are disagreeable; they might even be pleasurable depending upon
mutual agreement and the circumstances. She speculates that,
compared to men, women's experience of sexuality is apt to be more diffuse
and less orgasm-centered, and therefore the nature of an erotic
relationship between a woman and a child cannot be understood from the
male perspective.
Sarrel and Masters (47)
note that sexual abuse of men by women has been an integral part of many
cultures, in which most of the sexual abuse has been committed by older
females on young males. This abuse has ranged from casual
masturbation to quiet an irritable child to long continued incestuous
relationships. They report on eleven case studies of males who have
sought treatment for sexual dysfunction that was a consequence of their
childhood sexual abuse by women. In four of the cases, the assaulted
males were physically constrained, violently attacked and feared for their
safety, but still functioned sexually. The authors observe that this
contradicts the belief that male cannot achieve or maintain an erection
when threatened or attacked by a woman. There is no information on
the characteristics of the perpetrators of the sexual assaults, but these
cases suggest that, although rare, women can commit rape and violent
sexual assault.
Finkelhor and Russell (6) and
Russell (8) discount the
hypothesized barriers to identification and conclude the research
indicates sexual contact between children and older women is a distinct
minority of child-adult sexual contacts. They believe the best estimates
put female perpetrators of sexual abuse at about 20 percent for male
children and five percent for female children. (Since this time,
however, Finkelhor has recently reported greater frequencies of females as
sexual abusers, as was discussed above.)
Although these proposed barriers are interesting, they are speculative
and not supported by empirical data. In summary, the different
studies report widely varied frequencies of sexual abuse by women.
The definition of sexual abuse used, the type of sample selected and the
methodology employed affects the results obtained. Although child
sexual abuse by females may not be as rare as was once believed, it does
occur. However, some of the recent studies may be inadvertently
including cases of false allegations.
CHARACTERISTICS OF FEMALE SEXUAL ABUSE PERPETRATORS
What are the circumstances under which women sexually abuse
children? What are the characteristics of such women? Are the
etiological factors similar to those in male perpetrated sexual abuse?
There are beginning to be studies which address these questions.
Many of these are based on small samples and case studies. Although
any generalizations from such reports must be made cautiously, this
information provides a necessary starting point in understanding female
sexual abuse perpetrators.
Mathews, Matthews, and Speltz (48),
Matthews, Mathews, and Speltz, (49)
and Patton (50), report on a study
of 16 female sexual offenders who were in the Genesis II treatment project
in Minnesota. All but one of the women studied were themselves
victims of childhood sexual abuse and many were also victims of physical
abuse. There were strong and consistent patterns of childhood social
isolation, alienation, and lack of development of interpersonal skills and
competence among perpetrators. Three categories of female sex
offenders were described: Teacher/Lover, Predisposed (intergenerational),
and Male-Coerced.
The Teacher/Lover is generally involved with prepubescent and
adolescent males with whom she relates as a peer. Her motive is,
ostensibly, to teach her young victims about sexuality.
The Predisposed offender is usually a victim of severe sexual abuse
that was initiated at a very young age and persisted over a long period of
time. She initiated the sexual abuse herself and the victims are her
own children. Her motives are nonthreatening emotional intimacy.
The Male-Coerced offender acts initially in conjunction with a male who
has previously abused children. She exhibits a pattern of extreme
dependency and nonassertive behavior and she may eventually initiate
sexual abuse herself. Her victims are children both within and
outside of the family.
Faller (51)
reports on a clinical sample of 40 women who were judged by staff to have
sexually abused at least 63 children. These women represented 14
percent of the total of 289 perpetrators of sexual abuse. Many of
the women had significant difficulties in psychological and social
functioning. About half had mental problems, both retardation and
psychotic illness. More than half had chemical dependency problems
and close to three-fourths had maltreated their victims in other ways in
addition to the sexual abuse. The women fell into five case types
(four were sexually abusive in more than one context).
1) |
Polyincestuous abuse: Twenty-nine (72.5 percent) of the
women fit into this category. In such cases, there are at
least two perpetrators and generally two or more victims.
Usually, a male rather than the female offender instigated the
abuse. The woman went along with the male and played a
secondary role. |
2) |
Single-parent abuse: Six (15 percent) of the women who
sexually abused were single parents. These mothers did not
have ongoing relationships with men and the oldest child seemed to
serve as a surrogate partner for the mother, often having adult
role responsibilities. |
3) |
Psychotic abusers: Only three (7.5 percent) of the women
were classified as psychotic at the time of the sexual
abuse. Therefore, this study does not support the clinical
assumption that most female perpetrators are highly disturbed and
often psychotic at the time of the sexual abuse. |
4) |
Adolescent perpetrators: Three (7.5 percent) were
adolescent girls who had difficulty with peer relationships and
lacked alternative sexual outlets. |
5) |
Noncustodial abusers: There was only one woman who was
the noncustodial mother of her victims and sexually abused them
during visitation. Faller believes that in such cases the
noncustodial parent is apt to be devastated at the loss of her
spouse and the children become the source of emotional
gratification. |
Faller concludes that the circumstances that lead women to sexually
abuse children can be differentiated from those causing men to do so.
McCarty (52)
describes the characteristics of 26 mother-child incest offenders.
These women were identified by the Dallas Incest Treatment Program over a
three-year period and constituted four percent of the offender
population. The cases had been validated by a protective service
investigation. Nine of the mothers were co-offenders with a male
partner, while 12 were independent offenders (a male offender was also
involved in half of these).
All but two of the women described their childhood as difficult and
abusive. When the mother was a co-offender, her dependency on her
spouse was the major contributing factor. Half of these women were
of borderline intelligence.
The independent offenders in particular were characterized as
experiencing themselves psychologically as loners and lacking any sense of
attachment or belonging. They were likely to have married as
teenagers. Half were characterized as seriously emotionally
disturbed and almost half had a serious chemical abuse problem.
However, all were at least of average intelligence. In three of the
cases of mother-son incest, the father was out of the home and the mothers
seemed to treat the boys as age mates. However, the women who abused
daughters seemed to treat the daughters as extensions of themselves.
Vander Mey (53)
reviews the research on sexually abused boys and reports that there is so
little information on sexual abuse of males that findings must be
considered tenuous. She tentatively posits that male incest victims
are abused more often by males than by females and that both mother and
father incest perpetrators tend to have emotional, social and
psychological problems compounded by poor impulse control, low self-esteem
and alcohol abuse.
Finkelhor (6, 54)
suggests that there are four components that contribute, in different
degrees and forms, to the making of a child molester. These four
components represent complementary processes which help explain the
diversity of the behavior of sexual abusers. These four factors are
sexual arousal, emotional congruence, blockage and disinhibition and
Finkelhor believes that examination of these factors can help explain why
sexual abusers are predominantly male.
Sexual arousal: In order for an adult to be aroused by a child,
there has to have been cultural or familial conditioning to sexual
activity with children or early fantasy reinforced by masturbation.
Emotional congruence: For emotional congruence, there is comfort
in relating to a child and satisfaction of emotional need through the
abuse. This is apt to be due to arrested development through limited
intelligence, immaturity, or low self-esteem.
Blockage: Age appropriate sexual opportunities may be blocked by
bad experiences with age appropriate adults, sexual dysfunction, limited
social skills, or marital disturbance.
Disinhibition: The abuser may lose control through impulse
control deficits, psychosis, alcohol, drugs, stress, or nonexistent family
rules.
Rowan et al. (37) describe
characteristics of the nine female (out of 600) sex offenders in terms of
Finkelhor's (6) four-factor model.
In five of the cases, the abuse occurred in conjunction with a dominant
male partner; in four the woman acted independently. The case
histories of several of the women showed a history of childhood abuse and
all had serious psychological problems or limited intelligence. The
four women who acted independently abused boys. Of the five who
acted in conjunction with a male, three had female victims, one a male
victim, and one victimized both a son and a daughter. The authors
conclude that none of these cases were true paraphilics according to the
DSM-III-R but that the female molesters did fit the model proposed by
Finkelhor.
James and Nasjleti (55), in discussing their clinical experience with
sexually abusive families, report that a minority of their cases involved female perpetrators.
Although the psychological profiles of these
mothers is sketchy, in general they have infantile and extreme dependency
needs, a marriage relationship that is absent or emotionally empty,
possessive and overprotective attitudes toward child victims, and alcohol
used as a crutch. These women expect their children to meet their
emotional needs and because of the mothers' traditional rule as a
caretaker, they are able to hide the sexually explosive nature of these
contacts.
Chasnoff et al. (56) report on three cases of sexual abuse by a mother
of her infant. The mothers all were separated from their sexual partners,
had demonstrated some confusion regarding sexual identity and had sought
assistance with chemical dependency during pregnancy. Two of the three
were diagnosed as borderline personality disorder and two had been raped.
All three women were isolated in their living arrangements and the authors
believe that the sexual abuse was motivated by loneliness. The social
alienation and isolation of the mothers were significant facts in the
molestation of their infants.
Goodwin and DiVasto (57) review six reported cases of mother-daughter
incest and two cases of grandmother-granddaughter incest. These cases
deviate from the usual descriptions of incest and the authors note that
physical closeness between mothers and daughters is less subject to taboo
than are father and daughter contacts. The greater toleration of physical
intimacy between mothers and daughters makes it more difficult for the
child, the parent, and eventually the therapist to recognize when these
contacts become incestuous. Although Goodwin and DiVasto acknowledge that
since the reports of mother-daughter incest are few and brief, any
conclusions must be tentative, they find the mothers seem to be similar to
those mothers who initiate mother-son incest. They describe the mothers as
aggressive women who have abandoned their maternal role for an exploitive
relationship with their children. Their need for nurturance precipitates a
sexual relationship with the child. In all five cases of mother-daughter
incest, the mothers were involved in deteriorating marriages. Goodwin and
DiVasto believe that mother-daughter incest is more common than the rare
case reports suggest.
Kempe and Kempe (58) suggest that with the high divorce rate, an
increasing number of boys are living alone with their mothers and become a
source of comfort and closeness which may sometimes substitute for the
companionship previously experienced in marriage. Although this in itself
is normal, it can lead to problem behaviors, such as taking the boy to bed
for comfort. Kempe and Kempe note that society is more ready to believe
that there is a sexual aspect to fathers who sleep with daughters compared
to mothers who sleep with sons. They describe two case studies in which
sleeping arrangements also included overt sexual behavior and state that
in their experience, the psychological effects to the boy can be
devastating.
Krug (59) reports on eight case histories of men who were sexually
abused by their mothers as children in which seven of the mothers slept
with their sons regularly until the boys were teenagers. The mothers, who
were either divorced or had troubled marriages, appeared to be trying to
satisfy emotional and relational needs through their sons. Some were
clearly socially insecure and isolated. In four of the cases, the mothers
initiated actual sexual contact, in the others there was no overt sexual
behavior. None of these mothers were described as psychotic.
Although these case studies are interesting, we question Krug's
classifying all of them as sexual abuse. The behaviors of the mothers
sleeping with their sons into adolescence may be inappropriate and may
infantilize the boys, but to label all such cases as sexual abuse is to
use a very inclusive definition of sexual abuse. Krug reports that all
these men had psychological and adjustment problems. However, since this
was a clinical sample we would expect the men to report emotional and
adjustment problems in that this is why they sought therapy.
In an early article describing different types of incest, Lukianowicz
(60) discusses five cases of female
perpetrators three mother-son and two
aunt-nephew. In one case of mother-son incest, the mother was a widow, and
in a second, the mother's married life was very unhappy. Both of these
women became very dependent on their eldest sons, in whom they saw the
idealized young lovers of their own youth. The third mother was a chronic
schizophrenic of low intelligence. One of the aunts was hypomanic and
seduced her nephew during a manic phase; the other was generally
promiscuous. Lukianowicz reports that in in many of the cases studied,
social isolation was a very important etiological factor.
Wakefield, Rogers and Underwager (61) describe four case histories of
women who had sexual contact with children. Each of these women had a
history of significant losses in her background along with a lack of
healthy, secure childhood relationships. The sexually abusive behavior was
triggered by a particular loss and in three of the cases the relationship
between the woman and the victim was emotional as well as sexual and
appeared to be engaged in primarily to satisfy the woman's emotional
needs.
O'Connor (62) reports on a group of 62 convicted and imprisoned female
sex offenders in Great Britain. In 39 (63 percent) of the sex offenses
with individual victims, the victims were children and in nine cases the
offender was the mother or stepmother. In most (25) of the cases the women
were convicted of aiding and abetting a male offender. Almost half of the
women convicted of child sexual abuse had a previous history of
psychiatric disorder. Sexual gratification was never noted as a motivation
for the women involved in sex offenses with a victim.
In another survey using a prison sample, Condy et al. (40), asked 625
female college students and 172 female prison inmates about their early
sexual contacts with younger males. They report that three (0.5 percent)
of the college women and thirteen (7.6 percent) of the prison women
reported that they had had sexual contact with a boy. These women were
more likely to have had early sexual experiences themselves (81 percent
compared to 21 percent) and were significantly higher on the schizophrenia
and hypomania scales and lower on the Lie scale on the Kincannon Mini-Mult.
The authors state that although they were atypical, the women were not psychotic.
Although this finding is consistent with other research
concerning the characteristics of females who sexually abuse children, the
different response set suggested by the lower Lie scale and the serious
limitations of the Kincannon (James Butcher, personal communication, l988)
must be considered. The authors also note the possible importance of the
lower socioeconomic status of the prisoners since it is well established
that acting out and illegal behaviors are more common among less
privileged segments of society.
Travin et al. (7) observe that only the most overt acts of sexual abuse
perpetrated by females are likely to come to the attention of the criminal
justice system. Also, female offenders often exhibit a history of
physical and sexual victimization, chronic substance abuse and
longstanding psychiatric disorder. Therefore, those female abusers
identified by the justice system as offenders generally represent women
who are both severe victims and victimizers. They report on nine case
histories of women referred for evaluation and treatment at a forensic
psychiatry clinic. These cases constituted 1 percent of the sex offenders
seen at their clinic. All nine women had backgrounds of severe
psychological, physical, and/or sexual abuse. E ach had low self-esteem,
reported few or no positive social contacts, and was functioning at a
marginal level. Four had histories of severe psychopathology, including
psychosis and substance abuse. The other five had obvious
characterological deficiencies, although no history of psychosis. Travin
et al. conclude that female sexual offenders do exist and may warrant the
diagnosis of paraphilic disorder.
CONCLUSIONS
Several conclusions can be drawn from the review of the literature on
female perpetrators:
1. |
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
of Mental Disorders (Third edition-revised)
()(). Washington, DC, 1987
[Back]
|
2. |
Arroyo W, Eth S, Pynoos R: Sexual assault of a mother by her
preadolescent son. The American Journal of Psychiatry
1984; 141:1107-1108 [Back]
|
3. |
Herman JL: Father-Daughter Incest (). Cambridge, MA,
Harvard University
Press, 1983 [Back]
|
4. |
Meiselman KC: Resolving the Trauma of Incest: Reintegration Therapy
with Survivors (). San Francisco,
Jossey-Bass, 1990
[Back]
|
5. |
Wahl CW: The psychodynamics of consummated maternal incest. Archives of General
Psychiatry 1960; 3:96/188-101/193 [Back]
|
6. |
Finkelhor D, Russell D: Women as perpetrators: review of the
evidence, in Child Sexual Abuse: New Theory and Research (). Edited by
Finkelhor D. New York, Free Press, 1984 [Back]
|
7. |
Travin S, Cullin K, Protter B: Female sex offenders: Severe victims
and victimizers. Journal of Forensic Sciences 1990; 35:140-150
[Back]
|
8. |
Finkelhor D: A Sourcebook on Child Sexual Abuse ()(). Beverly Hills,
Sage, 1986
[Back]
|
9. |
Dawes RM: Rational Choice in an Uncertain World (). New York,
Harcourt Brace
Jovanovich, 1988 [Back]
|
10. |
Loftus EF, Korf NL, Schooler JW: Misguided memories: sincere
distortion of reality, in Credibility Assessment (). Edited by Yuille JC.
Boston, Kluwer Academic
Publishers, 1989 [Back]
|
11. |
Loftus E, Ketcham K: Witness for the Defense ()(). New York,
St.
Martin's Press, 1991 [Back]
|
12. |
Raphael KG, Cloitre M, Dohrenwend BP: Problems of recall and
misclassification with checklist methods of measuring stressful life
events. Health Psychology
1991; 10:62-74 [Back]
|
13. |
Gerlsma C, Emmelkamp PM, Arrindell WA: Anxiety, depression and
perception of early parenting: a meta-analysis. Clinical
Psychology Review 1990; 10:251-277 [Back]
|
14. |
Green BF, Hall JA: Quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annual Review of
Psychology 1984; 35,37-53 [Back]
|
15. |
Ross MW: Retrospective distortions in homosexual research. Archives of Sexual Behavior
1980; 9:523-531 [Back]
|
16. |
Wakefield H, Underwager R: Accusations of Child Sexual Abuse ()().
Springfield, IL, C. C. Thomas, 1988
[Back]
|
17. |
Okami P: Sociopolitical biases in the contemporary scientific
literature on adult human sexual behavior with children and adolescents,
in Pedophilia: Biosocial Dimensions (). Edited by Feierman JR. New York,
Springer-Verlag, 1990
[Back]
|
17a. |
Finkelhor D: Sexually Victimized Children (). New York, Free Press, 1979
[Back]
|
18. |
Russell D: The Secret Trauma: Incest in the Lives of Girls and Women (). New York,
Basic Books, 1986
[Back]
|
19. |
Sears DO: College sophomores in the laboratory: influence of a
narrow data base on social psychology's view of human nature. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 1986; 51:515-530 [Back]
|
20. |
Peters JM: Why prosecute child abuse? Paper presented at the
Seventeenth Annual Child Abuse and Neglect Symposium, Keystone, Colorado,
June 1988 [Back]
|
21. |
Dawes RM: Experience and validity of clinical judgment: the
illusory correlation. Behavioral Sciences
and the Law 1989; 7:457-467 [Back]
|
22. |
Dawes RM, Faust D, Meehl PE: Clinical versus actuarial judgment. Science
1989: 243:1668-1674 [Back]
|
23. |
Finkelhor D, Williams LM, Burns N: Nursery Crimes: Sexual Abuse in
Day Care (). Newbury Park,
Sage, 1988
[Back]
|
24. |
Finkelhor D, Williams LM, Burns N, Kalinowski M: Sexual abuse in day care: a national study. Study prepared under grant 90-CA-1155
from the National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect. University of New Hampshire,
Family Research Laboratory, 1988
[Back]
|
25. |
Coleman, L: Book review of Finkelhor D, Williams LM, Burns N: Nursery crimes: sexual abuse in day care. Issues in Child abuse
Accusations 1989; 1:3:46
[Back]
|
26. |
Lanning KV: Child Sex Rings: A Behavioral Analysis. Arlington, VA, National Center for Missing and Exploited
Children, 1989 [Back]
|
27. |
Faust D: Data integration in legal evaluations: can clinicians
deliver on their premises? Behavioral Sciences
and the Law 1989; 7:469-483 [Back]
|
28. |
Meehl PE: Some ruminations on the validation of clinical
procedures. Canadian Journal of
Psychology 1959; 13:102-128 [Back]
|
29. |
Meehl PE: Law and the fireside inductions (with postscript): some
reflections of a clinical psychologist. Behavioral Sciences
and
the Law 1989; 7:521-550 [Back]
|
30. |
Meehl P: If Freud could define psychoanalysis, why can't ABPP do
it? Paper presented at the 98th annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, August 10-14, 1990, Boston, MA
[Back]
|
31. |
Faller KC: Characteristics of a clinical sample of sexually abused
children: how boy and girl victims differ. Child Abuse and Neglect 1989; 13:281-291
[Back]
|
32. |
Fritz G, Stoll K, Wagner N: A comparison of males and females who
were sexually molested as children. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy
1981; 7:54-58 [Back]
|
33. |
Risin LI, Koss MP: The sexual abuse of boys: prevalence and
descriptive characteristics of childhood victimizations.
Journal of Interpersonal Violence 1987; 2:309-323 [Back]
|
34. |
Fromuth ME, Burkhart BR: Childhood sexual victimization among
college men: definitional and methodological issues. Violence and Victims
1987; 2:241-253 [Back]
|
35. |
Reinhart MA: Sexually abused boys. Child Abuse and Neglect 1987; 11:229-235
[Back]
|
36. |
Farber ED, Showers J, Johnson CF, Joseph JA, Oshins, L: The sexual
abuse of children: a comparison of male and female victims. Journal of Clinical
and Child
Psychology 1984; 13:294-297 [Back]
|
37. |
Rowan EL, Rowan JB, Langelier P: Women who molest children. Bull Am Acad Psychiatry and Law 1990; 18,79-83
[Back]
|
38. |
Johnson RL, Shrier D: Past sexual victimization by females of male
patients in an adolescent medicine clinic population. American Journal of Psychiatry
1987; 144:650-138 [Back]
|
39. |
Petrovich M, Templer DI: Heterosexual molestation of children who
later became rapists. Psychological Reports 1984; 54:810 [Back]
|
40. |
Condy SR, Templer DI, Brown R, Veaco L: Parameters of sexual
contact of boys with women. Archives of Sexual Behavior
1987; 16:379-394 [Back]
|
41. |
Bolton FG, Morris LA, MacEachron AE: Males at Risk: The Other Side
of Sexual Abuse (). Newbury Park, CA,
Sage, 1989
[Back]
|
42. |
Allen CM: Women as perpetrators of child sexual abuse: recognition
barriers, in The Incest Perpetrator: A family Member No One Wants to Treat
(). Edited by A. L. Horton AL, Johnson BL, Roundy LM, Williams D.
Newbury Park, Sage, 1990
[Back]
|
43. |
Bolton FG, Bolton SR: Working with Violent Families: A Guide for
Clinical and Legal Practitioners ()(). Newbury Park, CA,
Sage, 1987
[Back]
|
44. |
Groth AN: Men Who Rape: The Psychology of the Offender (). New York,
Plenum Press, 1979
[Back]
|
45. |
Banning A: Mother-son incest: confronting a prejudice. Child Abuse
and Neglect 1989; 13:563-570 [Back]
|
46. |
Sax M: Women and pedophilia. Unpublished paper [Back]
|
47. |
Sarrel PM, Masters WH: Sexual molestation of men by women. Archives of Sexual Behavior
1982;2:l 17-131 [Back]
|
48. |
Mathews R, Matthews JK, Speltz K: Female Sexual Offenders: An
Exploratory Study (). Orwell, VT,
The Safer Society
Press, 1989 [Back]
|
49. |
Matthews JK, Mathews R, Speltz K: Female sexual offenders: a
typology, in Family Sexual Abuse: Frontline Research and Evaluation ()().
Edited by Patton MQ. Newbury Park, Sage, 1991
[Back]
|
50. |
Patton MQ: Family sexual abuse project: overview and synthesis of
two years of research findings. Unpublished manuscript [Back]
|
51. |
Faller KC: Women who sexually abuse children. Violence and Victims 1987; 2:263-276
[Back]
|
52. |
McCarty LM: Mother-child incest: characteristics of the offender.
Child Welfare 1986; 55:447-458 [Back]
|
53. |
Vander Mey BJ: The sexual victimization of male children: a review
of previous research. Child Abuse and Neglect 1988; 12:61-72 [Back]
|
54. |
Araji S, Finkelhor D: Abusers: A review of the research, in
Finkelhor D: A Sourcebook on Child Sexual Abuse ()(). Beverly Hills,
Sage, 1986
[Back]
|
55. |
James B, Nasjleti M: Treating Sexually Abused Children and their
Families (). Palo Alto, CA,
Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc., 1983
[Back]
|
56. |
Chasnoff IJ, Burns WJ, Schnoll SH, Burns K, Chisum G, Kyle-Sproe L:
Maternal-neonatal incest. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry
1986; 56:577-580 [Back]
|
57. |
Goodwin J, DiVasto P: Female homosexuality: a sequel to
mother-daughter incest, in Sexual Abuse: Incest Victims and their Families
(2nd ed.) ()(). Edited by Goodwin JM. Chicago, Year Book Medical Publishers,
Inc., 1989 [Back]
|
58. |
Kempe RS, Kempe CH: The Common Secret: Sexual Abuse of Children and
Adolescents
(). New York,
W.H. Freeman and
Company, 1984 [Back]
|
59. |
Krug R.S: Adult male report of childhood sexual abuse by mothers: Case descriptions, motivations and long-term consequences. Child Abuse
and Neglect 1989; 13:111-120 [Back]
|
60. |
Lukianowicz N: Incest. I: paternal incest; II: other types of
incest. British Journal of Psychiatry
1972; 120:301-313 [Back]
|
61. |
Wakefield H, Rogers M, Underwager R: Female Sexual Abusers: A
Theory of Loss. Issues in Child Abuse Accusations 1990;
2:4:181-195
[Back]
|
62. |
O'Connor AA: Female sex offenders. British Journal of Psychiatry
1987; 150:615-620 [Back]
|